How to Edit and draw up Scoring Assistant Sample Report Online
Read the following instructions to use CocoDoc to start editing and signing your Scoring Assistant Sample Report:
- To begin with, find the “Get Form” button and tap it.
- Wait until Scoring Assistant Sample Report is loaded.
- Customize your document by using the toolbar on the top.
- Download your finished form and share it as you needed.
The Easiest Editing Tool for Modifying Scoring Assistant Sample Report on Your Way


How to Edit Your PDF Scoring Assistant Sample Report Online
Editing your form online is quite effortless. It is not necessary to get any software through your computer or phone to use this feature. CocoDoc offers an easy tool to edit your document directly through any web browser you use. The entire interface is well-organized.
Follow the step-by-step guide below to eidt your PDF files online:
- Browse CocoDoc official website on your device where you have your file.
- Seek the ‘Edit PDF Online’ button and tap it.
- Then you will open this tool page. Just drag and drop the document, or upload the file through the ‘Choose File’ option.
- Once the document is uploaded, you can edit it using the toolbar as you needed.
- When the modification is completed, click on the ‘Download’ icon to save the file.
How to Edit Scoring Assistant Sample Report on Windows
Windows is the most conventional operating system. However, Windows does not contain any default application that can directly edit file. In this case, you can get CocoDoc's desktop software for Windows, which can help you to work on documents quickly.
All you have to do is follow the steps below:
- Install CocoDoc software from your Windows Store.
- Open the software and then drag and drop your PDF document.
- You can also drag and drop the PDF file from Dropbox.
- After that, edit the document as you needed by using the different tools on the top.
- Once done, you can now save the finished document to your device. You can also check more details about how to alter a PDF.
How to Edit Scoring Assistant Sample Report on Mac
macOS comes with a default feature - Preview, to open PDF files. Although Mac users can view PDF files and even mark text on it, it does not support editing. With the Help of CocoDoc, you can edit your document on Mac instantly.
Follow the effortless steps below to start editing:
- Firstly, install CocoDoc desktop app on your Mac computer.
- Then, drag and drop your PDF file through the app.
- You can upload the file from any cloud storage, such as Dropbox, Google Drive, or OneDrive.
- Edit, fill and sign your template by utilizing this amazing tool.
- Lastly, download the file to save it on your device.
How to Edit PDF Scoring Assistant Sample Report on G Suite
G Suite is a conventional Google's suite of intelligent apps, which is designed to make your workforce more productive and increase collaboration within teams. Integrating CocoDoc's PDF file editor with G Suite can help to accomplish work handily.
Here are the steps to do it:
- Open Google WorkPlace Marketplace on your laptop.
- Look for CocoDoc PDF Editor and install the add-on.
- Upload the file that you want to edit and find CocoDoc PDF Editor by clicking "Open with" in Drive.
- Edit and sign your template using the toolbar.
- Save the finished PDF file on your device.
PDF Editor FAQ
What does a surveillance nurse do?
Better patient outcomes are often achieved through effective surveillance, a primary function of nurses. The purpose of this paper is to define, operationalize, measure, and evaluate the nurse surveillance capacity of hospitals. Nurse surveillance capacity is defined as the organizational features that enhance or weaken nurse surveillance. It includes a set of registered nurses (staffing, education, expertise, experience) and nurse practice environment characteristics.Empirical referents were extracted from existing survey data from 9,232 nurses in 174 hospitals. Using a ranking methodology, a Hospital Nurse Surveillance Capacity Profile was created for each hospital. Greater nurse surveillance capacity was significantly associated with a better quality of care and fewer adverse events. The profile may assist administrators to improve nurse surveillance and patient outcomes.Surveillance has multiple meanings in health care. The most familiar is population-based monitoring of health indicators, such as tracking infectious disease vectors or documenting increasing obesity rates in the general population. Nurses may be involved in population health surveillance through activities such as blood pressure and weight screenings at health fairs and in epidemiological studies.There is another less frequently used meaning of surveillance that involves the health care of individuals. Dougherty (1999) defined this type of surveillance as “the application of behavioral and cognitive processes in the systematic collection of information used to make judgments and predictions about a person’s health status”.In addition, in the Nursing Interventions Classification (NIC), surveillance is defined as “the purposeful and ongoing acquisition, interpretation, and synthesis of patient data for clinical decision-making” (McCloskey & Bulechek, 1996).A conceptualization has not been developed, however, that captures the essence of nurse surveillance as a cumulative and temporal process, or describes a healthcare organization’s capacity for excellent nurse surveillance.The Quality Health Outcomes Model (QHOM; Mitchell, Ferketich, & Jennings, 1998) provided a theoretical base for framing the examination of nurse surveillance and nurse surveillance capacity. In the model, system and patient characteristics have a direct effect on outcomes; the effect of an intervention on outcomes is mediated by both system and patient characteristics. In the context of our study, nurse surveillance capacity has a direct effect on outcomes, and the effect of nurse surveillance on outcomes is mediated by nurse surveillance capacity and patient characteristics. The system characteristic in our study is nurse surveillance capacity (i.e., RN characteristics and the nurse practice environment) that influences the intervention of nurse surveillance. A direct measure of nurse surveillance is difficult to obtain; therefore our study does not show how the relationship between the nurse surveillance and outcomes could be mediated by system characteristics. Rather, the focus of this study was on the direct relationship between nurse surveillance capacity and outcomes, including quality of care. Patient characteristics, such as the severity of illness at admission, also are theorized to affect patient outcomes. However, the inclusion of patient characteristics was not possible because patient-level data were not collected in the survey.Nurse Surveillance: An InterventionNurse surveillance is a process through which nurses monitor, evaluate, and act upon emerging indicators of a patient’s change in status. The components of this process include ongoing observation and assessment, recognition, interpretation of clinical data, and decision-making.Ongoing observation and assessmentTemporality is a critical component in the examination of surveillance (Dougherty, 1999). According to Dougherty, surveillance differs from assessment in that surveillance is an ongoing practice that occurs over time, whereas assessment frequently is referred to as a one-time event. The process of ongoing observation and data collection in nursing includes physical and mental examinations, and vigilant watching for physiological or behavioral changes using sensory data, such as seeing and hearing, during interactions with patients (Zeitz, 2005).Technological devices, such as electronic monitors, may aid nurses in this function as well. Nurse surveillance also includes the ongoing monitoring of laboratory findings and medications, including side effects and drug interactions (Benson & Briscoe, 2003). As a consequence of surveillance, changes in patient status are identified either as presenting a risk or as evidence of desired responses to treatment.RecognitionAn integral piece of surveillance is the nurse’s ability to recognize patient conditions that deviate from baseline measurements or parameters of interest. The parameters of interest are established through the initial assessment of the patient (Dougherty, 1999). Several indicators serve as markers for recognizable change in patient status including vital signs, neurological and mental status, cardiac and respiratory functioning, and laboratory results (IOM, 2004). The ability to recognize and “read the situation” requires professional knowledge, expertise, and experience. This skill is facilitated by a nurse’s ability to recall previous experiences of similar situations and respond (Benner & Tanner, 1987).InterpretationAfter observing and recognizing an alteration, the nurse interprets and synthesizes this information in the context of the patient and the environment, relying heavily upon critical thinking and clinical judgment. Critical thinking entails an attitude of skeptical inquiry as well as intellectual ability (Kenney, 1995). Tanner, Benner, Chesla, and Gordon (1993) defined clinical judgment as to the application of formal knowledge and theory to nurses’ understanding of patients in the context of a given situation.Decision-makingAfter collecting and interpreting patient data, the nurse makes a decision to continue monitoring or act upon indicators of change in a patient’s status. Nurses act upon changes in a patient’s status by modifying the plan of care, communicating with other providers, or mobilizing resources. Multiple researchers (Kramer et al., 2007; Kramer & Schmalenberg, 2004a; Pearson et al., 2000) have cited the importance of nurses’ independent decision making to the quality of care that patients receive. Often, decisions may be influenced by organizational and environmental factors, such as resource availability and the practice environment.Cumulative and Temporal Aspects of SurveillanceAlthough surveillance is considered to be a nursing intervention provided by a single nurse on behalf of a single patient, in reality, individual patients are cared for in most contexts by multiple nurses over time. Thus, nurse surveillance as a nursing intervention has cumulative and temporal aspects.Associating the effectiveness of surveillance by an individual nurse with the outcomes of an individual patient in most contemporary health care settings is not possible because nurse surveillance is cumulative across nurses and overtime. Benner (1984, p. 126) illustrated this in her qualitative research reported in From Novice to Expert: a charge nurse making rounds enters a room and immediately observes a lidocaine intravenous drip using a macro- rather than a micro-drip. Nurses on two previous shifts, including a float nurse and a new graduate, had failed to associate the patient’s lethargy to over-medication. The charge nurse, whose surveillance was excellent, instituted a rescue attempt by turning the drip off, but the collective surveillance across multiple nurses over time contributed to the patient’s subsequent cardiac arrest and death. Thus, nurse surveillance is a collective effort of interventions delivered by multiple nurses over time, as well as interventions by individual nurses.It is equally difficult to measure individual and collective nurse surveillance. However, an organizational level indicator of nurse surveillance capacity across nurses and overtime would offer a metric with the potential to guide decisions that could improve surveillance, quality of care, and patient outcomes. A metric such as a nurse surveillance capacity also would be valuable to build research evidence for administrators.Nurse Surveillance CapacityNurse surveillance capacity is defined as the organizational features that enhance or weaken nurse surveillance. Nurse surveillance capacity includes RN characteristics (staffing, education, clinical expertise, and years of experience), and the nurse practice environment. The concurrent evaluation of each of these characteristics comprises the Hospital Nurse Surveillance Capacity Profile.StaffingRegistered nurse staffing has been associated empirically with patient outcomes (Aiken, Clarke, Sloane, Sochalski, & Silber, 2002; Mark, Harless, McCue, & Xu, 2004; Needleman, Buerhaus, Mattke, Stewart, & Zelevinsky, 2002). For example, Aiken, Clarke, Sloane, Sochalski, et al (2002) found an increased risk of 30-day mortality, as well as an increased risk of failure-to-rescue, for surgical patients in hospitals with high patient-to-nurse ratios. Therefore, “the effectiveness of nurse surveillance is influenced by the number of registered nurses available to assess patients on an ongoing basis” (Aiken, Clarke, Sloane, Sochalski, et al., 2002, p. 1992).EducationIn addition to staffing, researchers have focused on the educational background of nurses as a predictor of patient outcomes (Aiken, Clarke, Cheung, Sloane, & Silber, 2003; Estabrooks, Midodzi, Cummings, Ricker, & Giovannetti, 2005; Tourangeau et al., 2007). Aiken and colleagues (2003) found significantly lower rates of 30-day mortality and failure-to-rescue among surgical patients in hospitals with higher proportions of nurses who had earned at least a baccalaureate degree in nursing. Aiken’s findings were supported in two studies in Canada (Estabrooks et al.; Tourangeau et al., 2007). Researchers hypothesize that baccalaureate preparation has a positive association with nurses’ critical thinking and clinical judgment skills, which are essential to the surveillance of patients, both in terms of risk for adverse events and in the evaluation of therapies (Aiken et al., 2003; Young, Lehrer, & White, 1991).Clinical expertiseIn an influential work, Benner & Tanner (1987) explored the relationship between expertise and nurses’ practice style and demonstrated how expert nurses develop intuition - a powerful feature of the surveillance process. Intuition is defined as “understanding without rationale” (Benner & Tanner, p. 23). Expert nurses are able to recognize patterns and relate current clinical situations to past experience, integrate the knowledge of the patient’s disease with contextual knowledge about the patient, and are skilled in their specialty areas.Expert nurses also practice deliberative rationality-the capacity to view a clinical scenario from different perspectives; they also possess a sense of salience- the ability to identify the most pertinent observations in a complex assessment (Benner & Tanner). Therefore, expert nurses are able to immediately detect changes in a patient’s condition and intervene to prevent adverse occurrences (Christensen & Hewitt-Taylor, 2006; Houser, 2003).Years of experienceExperience is necessary for gaining expertise, but the two are not necessarily interchangeable (Christensen & Hewitt-Taylor, 2006; Houser, 2003). The number of years of experience, however, provides exposure to different patient conditions and clinical scenarios that contribute to the development of knowledge, technical skills, and critical thinking (Benner, 1984; Newman, 1990).Research that links experience to patient outcomes is scarce; however, existing findings are promising. For example, Tourangeau, Giovannetti, Tu, and Wood (2002) found that each additional year of nurse experience was associated with six fewer patient deaths for every 1000 patients discharged from urban community hospitals. In another study, units with more experienced nurses reported lower rates of medication errors and adverse events (Blegen, Vaughn, & Goode, 2001).Nurse practice environmentThe nurse practice environment is defined as “the organizational characteristics of a work setting that facilitate or constrain professional nursing practice” (Lake, 2002, p. 178). Several instruments have been developed to measure the nurse practice environment.One of these, the Practice Environment Scale of the Nursing Work Index (PES-NWI; Lake, 2002), is used to measure elements that are critical to nurse surveillance. Furthermore, the PES-NWI has been endorsed by the National Quality Forum (NQF) as a nursing-sensitive standard measure for inpatient care (NQF, 2004).The five domains of the PES-NWI are:Nurse Participation in Hospital Affairs;Nursing Foundations for Quality of Care;Nurse Manager Ability,Leadership and Support of Nurses;Staffing and Resource Adequacy; andCollegial Nurse-Physician Relations (Lake, 2002).Nurse participation in hospital affairs is not an obvious component of surveillance. However, in hospitals where nurses are able to influence administrative decisions and policies, nurses are likely to be more efficient in their practice (Aiken, Lake, Sochalski, & Sloane, 1997).Nursing foundations for quality care, such as patient assignments that promote continuity of care, are conducive to surveillance by enhancing the accumulation of knowledge about a patient. Job performance and productivity, including surveillance, depending on the support and ability of the nurse manager (Kramer, Schmalenberg, & Maguire, 2004).Nurses need the staffing support to have adequate time to spend with their patients to perform surveillance, as well as adequate resources available to them to implement necessary interventions (Laschinger & Lieter, 2006; McCusker, Dendukuri, Cardinal, Laplante, & Bambonye, 2004). Further, collegial relationships with physicians are essential to effective surveillance through the exchange of information vital to the patient’s clinical condition (Baggs et al., 1999; Kramer & Schmalenberg, 2004b).Nurse Surveillance and OutcomesBetter nurse surveillance is considered to be the link between better RN staffing/education and the prevention of mortality and failure-to-rescue in surgical patients (Aiken et al., 2003; Aiken, Clarke, Sloane, Sochalski, et al., 2002; Clarke & Aiken, 2003). With the elements of nurse surveillance capacity in place, nurses are better able to perform adequate surveillance of patients and initiate a timely response when a complication or adverse event is detected.Therefore, theoretically, multiple adverse outcomes may be used to assess the effectiveness of hospital nurse surveillance capacity, including mortality and failure-to-rescue. We posit that nurse surveillance capacity has the same relevance for all patient populations and non-mortality outcomes. Other outcomes of nurse surveillance capacity may include quality of care and adverse events, such as falls and nosocomial infections.Data SourceThis study was a secondary analysis of data derived from a 50% random sample survey of Pennsylvania RNs that was conducted in 1999. The survey response rate was 52%, for a total of 43,329 RNs (Aiken et al., 2001). The survey was developed to examine the relationships between nurse staffing, work environment, and patient outcomes (Aiken, Clarke, & Sloane, 2002). The survey contained questions about hospital work environments, workload, workplace safety, quality of care assessments, demographics, education, and experience. Data reflecting individual patient characteristics were not collected in the survey.SampleOf the 43,329 respondents, 13,204 nurses indicated they worked in one of the 210 acute care hospitals in the state. Nurses were included in the final sample for this study if they identified their primary position as a staff nurse working in acute care (excluding the emergency room). Hospitals were included in the sample that had a sufficient number of respondents to assure reliable aggregate values of surveillance capacity variables, as described elsewhere (Aiken, Clarke, Sloane, Sochalski, et al., 2002). An average of 60 nurse respondents from each hospital completed questionnaires. One-half of the hospitals had more than 50 nurse respondents and over 80% of the hospitals had more than 25 nurse respondents. The final sample for this analysis consisted of 9,232 RNs in 174 hospitals in the state of Pennsylvania.MeasuresEmpirical referents were extracted from the Pennsylvania nurse survey to operationalize nurse surveillance capacity. A Hospital Nurse Surveillance Capacity Profile was constructed for each hospital by ranking hospitals on a set of RN characteristics (staffing, education, clinical expertise, and years of experience) and the nurse practice environment. An aggregate measure of hospital nurse surveillance capacity was constructed by calculating a hospital’s average ranking across indicators. Nurse-assessed quality of care and two adverse event variables (nosocomial infections and patient falls with injuries) were the outcomes in this study.StaffingNurses were asked to provide the number of patients cared for on their last shift. The mean number of patients cared for was calculated across all staff nurses within a hospital who reported caring for at least 1 but no more than 20 patients on their last shift. Aiken, Clarke, Sloane, Sochalski, and Silber (2002) considered this measure of staffing to be more accurate than administrative database sources. Harless and Mark (2006) discussed the bias introduced into staffing estimates when using administrative financial data to predict staffing allocation levels.EducationNurses were asked to provide their highest level of education in nursing. Respondents chose from diploma, associate degree, baccalaureate degree, master’s degree, or other. The education variable was operationalized as the proportion of staff nurse respondents within each hospital holding a baccalaureate degree or higher.Clinical expertiseNurses were asked to describe their clinical nursing expertise by choosing from one of the following response categories defined by Benner (1984): advanced beginner, competent, proficient, and expert. Responses were coded 1 to 4 respectively. The clinical expertise variable was calculated as the mean expertise of nurse respondents in each hospital.Years of experienceNurses were asked to provide the number of years they had worked as an RN. The experience variable was calculated at the hospital-level as the mean number of years of RN experience.Nurse practice environmentThe nurse practice environment was measured using the PES-NWI (Lake, 2002). The PES-NWI includes 31 items that use a 4-point Likert scale (strongly agree, somewhat agree, somewhat disagree, and strongly disagree) to assess nurses’ perceptions of the presence of organizational characteristics in their hospitals.A mean score for each of the five subscales was calculated at the hospital-level from the hospital mean of the individual items composing each subscale (Rousseau, 1985; Verran, Gerber, & Milton, 1995).The reliability of the five subscales was examined at the hospital-level by calculating the intraclass correlation coefficient (ICC (1, k)) across hospitals using a minimum criterion of .60 (Glick, 1985).All five subscales demonstrated acceptable reliability at the hospital-level with ICC (1, k)s ranging from a low of .67 for the Collegial Nurse-Physician Relations subscale to a high of .89 for the Participation in Hospital Affairs subscale. Internal consistency coefficients (Cronbach’s alphas) for the five subscales ranged from .78 to .85.Discriminate validity of the PES-NWI has been demonstrated by its ability to detect differences in the nurse practice environments of a magnet and non-magnet hospitals (Lake & Friese, 2006).Quality of careNurses were asked to rate the quality of nursing care delivered to patients on their unit using a 4-point Likert scale of excellent, good, fair, and poor. The reliability of the quality of care variable was examined at the aggregate level by calculating the ICC (1, k) across hospitals. The ICC (1, k) for quality of care was .73. Responses were collapsed into categories of poor/fair and good/excellent care to examine associations with the nurse surveillance capacity rankings.Adverse eventsNurses were asked to report how often two different adverse events, nosocomial infections, and patient falls with injuries, occurred involving their patients over the past year. The frequency of adverse events was measured using a 4-point Likert-type scale of never, rarely, occasionally, or frequently. The reliability of the adverse event measures at the aggregate level was examined by calculating the ICC (1, k) across hospitals.The ICC (1, k)s for nosocomial infections and patient falls with injuries were .73 and .71, respectively. Responses were collapsed into categories of never/rarely and occasionally/frequently to examine associations with the nurse surveillance capacity rankings.Data AnalysisThe purposes of the data analysis were to describe hospitals’ nurse surveillance capacity, to rank hospitals by nurse surveillance capacity, and to associate hospitals’ average ranking with quality of care indicators. Individual nurse responses were examined to assess demographics. Nurse responses were then aggregated to the hospital-level to create the nurse surveillance capacity indicators. Distributions for the aggregated variables were calculated.To create a Hospital Nurse Surveillance Capacity Profile, the hospitals first were ranked on each nurse surveillance capacity indicator so that they rank on each indicator reflected the hospital’s placement among all hospitals. A profile was developed to display hospital ranks by deciles for clarity of comparison, with 1 as the lowest and 10 as the highest decile. A Hospital Nurse Surveillance Capacity Profile was created for each of the 174 represented hospitals.Hospitals’ overall nurse surveillance capacity was then calculated as the mean ranking across all nine nurse surveillance capacity indicators. Finally, hospitals’ overall nurse surveillance capacities were ordered into deciles, with 1 as the lowest and 10 as the highest decile. One hospital profile from the overall highest decile and another from the lowest were selected to illustrate and to contrast a highly ranked hospital and its consistent performance across indicators with a low ranked hospital.To associate hospitals’ rankings with quality of care indicators, outcomes of hospitals in the highest and lowest deciles of overall nurse surveillance capacity ranking were compared. Nurse responses were aggregated to the hospital-level to create the outcome measures.Same-source bias was of concern as the nurse survey was used to measure the independent and dependent variables. Therefore, a split-sample approach was used to test the robustness of the estimates. In every hospital, a random half of the nurses’ responses were aggregated for the organizational measures. The other half was aggregated for the outcome measures, and the analysis was repeated.FindingsNurse Respondent DemographicsDemographics of the nurse respondents were examined. Nearly all respondents were female (94%). The average respondent was 39 years of age. The majority of the nurses worked on a medical/surgical unit (38%); about a quarter of the respondents (24%) worked in intensive care.Nurse Surveillance Capacity DistributionsAverage staffing across all hospitals was just over 5 patients per nurse. On average, one-third of the nurses within a hospital held at least a baccalaureate degree had over 13 years of experience as an RN and rated themselves as competent to proficient in their clinical expertise.A low proportion of hospitals (1 in10) had a majority of nurses who rated their practice as proficient or expert (average expertise ≥3.0). Hsubscale hospitals tended to score highest on the Nursing Foundations for Quality of Care of the PES-NWI; the lowest scored subscale was Staffing and Resource Adequacy. The Quality Health Outcomes Model (QHOM; Mitchell, Ferketich, & Jennings, 1998) provided a theoretical base for framing the examination of nurse surveillance and nurse surveillance capacity.In the model, system and patient characteristics have a direct effect on outcomes; the effect of an intervention on outcomes is mediated by both system and patient characteristics. In the context of our study, nurse surveillance capacity has a direct effect on outcomes, and the effect of nurse surveillance on outcomes is mediated by nurse surveillance capacity and patient characteristics. The system characteristic in our study is nurse surveillance capacity (i.e., RN characteristics and the nurse practice environment) that influences the intervention of nurse surveillance.A direct measure of nurse surveillance is difficult to obtain; therefore our study does not show how the relationship between the nurse surveillance and outcomes could be mediated by system characteristics. Rather, the focus of this study was on the direct relationship between nurse surveillance capacity and outcomes, including quality of care. Patient characteristics, such as the severity of illness at admission, also are theorized to affect patient outcomes. However, the inclusion of patient characteristics was not possible because patient-level data were not collected in the survey.Nurse Surveillance: An InterventionNurse surveillance is a process through which nurses monitor, evaluate, and act upon emerging indicators of a patient’s change in status. The components of this process include ongoing observation and assessment, recognition, interpretation of clinical data, and decision-making.Ongoing observation and assessmentTemporality is a critical component in the examination of surveillance (Dougherty, 1999). According to Dougherty, surveillance differs from assessment in that surveillance is an ongoing practice that occurs over time, whereas assessment frequently is referred to as a one-time event.The process of ongoing observation and data collection in nursing includes physical and mental examinations, and vigilant watching for physiological or behavioral changes using sensory data, such as seeing and hearing, during interactions with patients (Zeitz, 2005).Technological devices, such as electronic monitors, may aid nurses in this function as well. Nurse surveillance also includes the ongoing monitoring of laboratory findings and medications, including side effects and drug interactions (Benson & Briscoe, 2003). As a consequence of surveillance, changes in patient status are identified either as presenting a risk or as evidence of desired responses to treatment.RecognitionAn integral piece of surveillance is the nurse’s ability to recognize patient conditions that deviate from baseline measurements or parameters of interest. The parameters of interest are established through the initial assessment of the patient (Dougherty, 1999).Several indicators serve as markers for recognizable change in patient status including vital signs, neurological and mental status, cardiac and respiratory functioning, and laboratory results (IOM, 2004). The ability to recognize and “read the situation” requires professional knowledge, expertise, and experience. This skill is facilitated by a nurse’s ability to recall previous experiences of similar situations and respond (Benner & Tanner, 1987).InterpretationAfter observing and recognizing an alteration, the nurse interprets and synthesizes this information in the context of the patient and the environment, relying heavily upon critical thinking and clinical judgment.Critical thinking entails an attitude of skeptical inquiry as well as intellectual ability (Kenney, 1995). Tanner, Benner, Chesla, and Gordon (1993) defined clinical judgment as to the application of formal knowledge and theory to nurses’ understanding of patients in the context of a given situation.Decision-makingAfter collecting and interpreting patient data, the nurse makes a decision to continue monitoring or act upon indicators of change in a patient’s status. Nurses act upon changes in a patient’s status by modifying the plan of care, communicating with other providers, or mobilizing resources.Multiple researchers (Kramer et al., 2007; Kramer & Schmalenberg, 2004a; Pearson et al., 2000) have cited the importance of nurses’ independent decision making to the quality of care that patients receive. Often, decisions may be influenced by organizational and environmental factors, such as resource availability and the practice environment.Cumulative and Temporal Aspects of SurveillanceAlthough surveillance is considered to be a nursing intervention provided by a single nurse on behalf of a single patient, in reality, individual patients are cared for in most contexts by multiple nurses over time. Thus, nurse surveillance as a nursing intervention has cumulative and temporal aspects.Associating the effectiveness of surveillance by an individual nurse with the outcomes of an individual patient in most contemporary health care settings is not possible because nurse surveillance is cumulative across nurses and overtime. Benner (1984, p. 126) illustrated this in her qualitative research reported in From Novice to Expert: a charge nurse making rounds enters a room and immediately observes a lidocaine intravenous drip using a macro- rather than a micro-drip.d fai Nure the patient’s lethargy to over-medication.The charge nurse, whose on two previous shifts, including a float nurse and a new graduate, haled to associate surveillance was excellent, instituted a rescue attempt by turning the drip off, but the collective surveillance across multiple nurses over time contributed to the patient’s subsequent cardiac arrest and death. Thus, nurse surveillance is a collective effort of interventions delivered by multiple nurses over time, as well as interventions by individual nurses.It is equally difficult to measure individual and collective nurse surveillance. However, an organizational level indicator of nurse surveillance capacity across nurses and overtime would offer a metric with the potential to guide decisions that could improve surveillance, quality of care, and patient outcomes. A metric such as nurse surveillance capacity also would be valuable to build research evidence for administrators.Nurse Surveillance CapacityNurse surveillance capacity is defined as the organizational features that enhance or weaken nurse surveillance. Nurse surveillance capacity includes RN characteristics (staffing, education, clinical expertise, and years of experience), and the nurse practice environment. The concurrent evaluation of each of these characteristics comprises the Hospital Nurse Surveillance Capacity Profile.StaffingRegistered nurse staffing has been associated empirically with patient outcomes (Aiken, Clarke, Sloane, Sochalski, & Silber, 2002; Mark, Harless, McCue, & Xu, 2004; Needleman, Buerhaus, Mattke, Stewart, & Zelevinsky, 2002). For example, Aiken, Clarke, Sloane, Sochalski, et al (2002) found an increased risk of 30-day mortality, as well as an increased risk of failure-to-rescue, for surgical patients in hospitals with high patient-to-nurse ratios. Therefore, “the effectiveness of nurse surveillance is influenced by the number of registered nurses available to assess patients on an ongoing basis” (Aiken, Clarke, Sloane, Sochalski, et al., 2002, p. 1992).EducationIn addition to staffing, researchers have focused on the educational background of nurses as a predictor of patient outcomes (Aiken, Clarke, Cheung, Sloane, & Silber, 2003; Estabrooks, Midodzi, Cummings, Ricker, & Giovannetti, 2005; Tourangeau et al., 2007). Aiken and colleagues (2003) found significantly lower rates of 30-day mortality and failure-to-rescue among surgical patients in hospitals with higher proportions of nurses who had earned at least a baccalaureate degree in nursing.Aiken’s findings were supported in two studies in Canada (Estabrooks et al.; Tourangeau et al., 2007). Researchers hypothesize that baccalaureate preparation has a positive association with nurses’ critical thinking and clinical judgment skills, which are essential to the surveillance of patients, both in terms of risk for adverse events and in the evaluation of therapies (Aiken et al., 2003; Young, Lehrer, & White, 1991).Clinical expertiseIn an influential work, Benner & Tanner (1987) explored the relationship between expertise and nurses’ practice style and demonstrated how expert nurses develop intuition - a powerful feature of the surveillance process. Intuition is defined as “understanding without rationale” (Benner & Tanner, p. 23).Expert nurses are able to recognize patterns and relate current clinical situations to past experience, integrate the knowledge of the patient’s disease with contextual knowledge about the patient, and are skilled in their specialty areas.Expert nurses also practice deliberative rationality-the capacity to view a clinical scenario from different perspectives; they also possess a sense of salience- the ability to identify the most pertinent observations in a complex assessment (Benner & Tanner). Therefore, expert nurses are able to immediately detect changes in a patient’s condition and intervene to prevent adverse occurrences (Christensen & Hewitt-Taylor, 2006; Houser, 2003).Years of experienceExperience is necessary for gaining expertise, but the two are not necessarily interchangeable (Christensen & Hewitt-Taylor, 2006; Houser, 2003). The number of years of experience, however, provides exposure to different patient conditions and clinical scenarios that contribute to the development of knowledge, technical skills, and critical thinking (Benner, 1984; Newman, 1990).Research that links experience to patient outcomes is scarce; however, existing findings are promising. For example, Tourangeau, Giovannetti, Tu, and Wood (2002) found that each additional year of nurse experience was associated with six fewer patient deaths for every 1000 patients discharged from urban community hospitals. In another study, units with more experienced nurses reported lower rates of medication errors and adverse events (Blegen, Vaughn, & Goode, 2001).Nurse practice environmentThe nurse practice environment is defined as “the organizational characteristics of a work setting that facilitate or constrain professional nursing practice” (Lake, 2002, p. 178). Several instruments have been developed to measure the nurse practice environment. One of these, the Practice Environment Scale of the Nursing Work Index (PES-NWI; Lake, 2002), is used to measure elements that are critical to nurse surveillance. Furthermore, the PES-NWI has been endorsed by the National Quality Forum (NQF) as a nursing-sensitive standard measure for inpatient care (NQF, 2004).The five domains of the PES-NWI are:Nurse Participation in Hospital Affairs;Nursing Foundations for Quality of Care;Nurse Manager Ability,Leadership and Support of Nurses;Staffing and Resource Adequacy; andCollegial Nurse-Physician Relations (Lake, 2002).Nurse participation in hospital affairs is not an obvious component of surveillance. However, in hospitals where nurses are able to influence administrative decisions and policies, nurses are likely to be more efficient in their practice (Aiken, Lake, Sochalski, & Sloane, 1997).Nursing foundations for quality care, such as patient assignments that promote continuity of care, are conducive to surveillance by enhancing the accumulation of knowledge about a patient.Job performance and productivity, including surveillance, depending on the support and ability of the nurse manager (Kramer, Schmalenberg, & Maguire, 2004).Nurses need the staffing support to have adequate time to spend with their patients to perform surveillance, as well as adequate resources available to them to implement necessary interventions (Laschinger & Lieter, 2006; McCusker, Dendukuri, Cardinal, Laplante, & Bambonye, 2004).Further, collegial relationships with physicians are essential to effective surveillance through the exchange of information vital to the patient’s clinical condition (Baggs et al., 1999; Kramer & Schmalenberg, 2004b).Nurse Surveillance and OutcomesBetter nurse surveillance is considered to be the link between better RN staffing/education and the prevention of mortality and failure-to-rescue in surgical patients (Aiken et al., 2003; Aiken, Clarke, Sloane, Sochalski, et al., 2002; Clarke & Aiken, 2003). With the elements of nurse surveillance capacity in place, nurses are better able to perform adequate surveillance of patients and initiate a timely response when a complication or adverse event is detected. Therefore, theoretically, multiple adverse outcomes may be used to assess the effectiveness of hospital nurse surveillance capacity, including mortality and failure-to-rescue. We posit that nurse surveillance capacity has the same relevance for all patient populations and non-mortality outcomes. Other outcomes of nurse surveillance capacity may include quality of care and adverse events, such as falls and nosocomial infections.MethodsData SourceThis study was a secondary analysis of data derived from a 50% random sample survey of Pennsylvania RNs that was conducted in 1999. The survey response rate was 52%, for a total of 43,329 RNs (Aiken et al., 2001). The survey was developed to examine the relationships between nurse staffing, work environment, and patient outcomes (Aiken, Clarke, & Sloane, 2002). The survey contained questions about hospital work environments, workload, workplace safety, quality of care assessments, demographics, education, and experience. Data reflecting individual patient characteristics were not collected in the survey.SampleOf the 43,329 respondents, 13,204 nurses indicated they worked in one of the 210 acute care hospitals in the state. Nurses were included in the final sample for this study if they identified their primary position as a staff nurse working in acute care (excluding the emergency room). Hospitals were included in the sample that had a sufficient number of respondents to assure reliable aggregate values of surveillance capacity variables, as described elsewhere (Aiken, Clarke, Sloane, Sochalski, et al., 2002). An average of 60 nurse respondents from each hospital completed questionnaires. One-half of the hospitals had more than 50 nurse respondents and over 80% of the hospitals had more than 25 nurse respondents. The final sample for this analysis consisted of 9,232 RNs in 174 hospitals in the state of Pennsylvania.MeasuresEmpirical referents were extracted from the Pennsylvania nurse survey to operationalize nurse surveillance capacity. A Hospital Nurse Surveillance Capacity Profile was constructed for each hospital by ranking hospitals on a set of RN characteristics (staffing, education, clinical expertise, and years of experience) and the nurse practice environment. An aggregate measure of hospital nurse surveillance capacity was constructed by calculating a hospital’s average ranking across indicators. Nurse-assessed quality of care and two adverse event variables (nosocomial infections and patient falls with injuries) were the outcomes in this study.StaffingNurses were asked to provide the number of patients cared for on their last shift. The mean number of patients cared for was calculated across all staff nurses within a hospital who reported caring for at least 1 but no more than 20 patients on their last shift. Aiken, Clarke, Sloane, Sochalski, and Silber (2002) considered this measure of staffing to be more accurate than administrative database sources. Harless and Mark (2006) discussed the bias introduced into staffing estimates when using administrative financial data to predict staffing allocation levels.EducationNurses were asked to provide their highest level of education in nursing. Respondents chose from diploma, associate degree, baccalaureate degree, master’s degree, or other. The education variable was operationalized as the proportion of staff nurse respondents within each hospital holding a baccalaureate degree or higher.Clinical expertiseNurses were asked to describe their clinical nursing expertise by choosing from one of the following response categories defined by Benner (1984): advanced beginner, competent, proficient, and expert. Responses were coded 1 to 4 respectively. The clinical expertise variable was calculated as the mean expertise of nurse respondents in each hospital.Years of experienceNurses were asked to provide the number of years they had worked as an RN. The experience variable was calculated at the hospital-level as the mean number of years of RN experience.Nurse practice environmentThe nurse practice environment was measured using the PES-NWI (Lake, 2002). The PES-NWI includes 31 items that use a 4-point Likert scale (strongly agree, somewhat agree, somewhat disagree, and strongly disagree) to assess nurses’ perceptions of the presence of organizational characteristics in their hospitals.A mean score for each of the five subscales was calculated at the hospital-level from the hospital mean of the individual items composing each subscale (Rousseau, 1985; Verran, Gerber, & Milton, 1995).The reliability of the five subscales was examined at the hospital-level by calculating the intraclass correlation coefficient (ICC (1, k)) across hospitals using a minimum criterion of .60 (Glick, 1985).All five subscales demonstrated acceptable reliability at the hospital-level with ICC (1, k)s ranging from a low of .67 for the Collegial Nurse-Physician Relations subscale to a high of .89 for the Participation in Hospital Affairs subscale. Internal consistency coefficients (Cronbach’s alphas) for the five subscales ranged from .78 to .85.Discriminate validity of the PES-NWI has been demonstrated by its ability to detect differences in the nurse practice environments of a magnet and non-magnet hospitals (Lake & Friese, 2006).Quality of careNurses were asked to rate the quality of nursing care delivered to patients on their unit using a 4-point Likert scale of excellent, good, fair, and poor. The reliability of the quality of care variable was examined at the aggregate level by calculating the ICC (1, k) across hospitals. The ICC (1, k) for quality of care was .73. Responses were collapsed into categories of poor/fair and good/excellent care to examine associations with the nurse surveillance capacity rankings.Adverse eventsNurses were asked to report how often two different adverse events, nosocomial infections, and patient falls with injuries, occurred involving their patients over the past year.The frequency of adverse events was measured using a 4-point Likert-type scale of never, rarely, occasionally, or frequently. The reliability of the adverse event measures at the aggregate level was examined by calculating the ICC (1, k) across hospitals. The ICC (1, k)s for nosocomial infections and patient falls with injuries were .73 and .71, respectively. Responses were collapsed into categories of never/rarely and occasionally/frequently to examine associations with the nurse surveillance capacity rankings.Data AnalysisThe purposes of the data analysis were to describe hospitals’ nurse surveillance capacity, to rank hospitals by nurse surveillance capacity, and to associate hospitals’ average ranking with quality of care indicators. Individual nurse responses were examined to assess demographics. Nurse responses were then aggregated to the hospital-level to create the nurse surveillance capacity indicators.Distributions for the aggregated variables were calculated. To create a Hospital Nurse Surveillance Capacity Profile, the hospitals first were ranked on each nurse surveillance capacity indicator so that they rank on each indicator reflected the hospital’s placement among all hospitals.A profile was developed to display hospital ranks by deciles for clarity of comparison, with 1 as the lowest and 10 as the highest decile. A Hospital Nurse Surveillance Capacity Profile was created for each of the 174 represented hospitals.Hospitals’ overall nurse surveillance capacity was then calculated as the mean ranking across all nine nurse surveillance capacity indicators. Finally, hospitals’ overall nurse surveillance capacities were ordered into deciles, with 1 as the lowest and 10 as the highest decile. One hospital profile from the overall highest decile and another from the lowest were selected to illustrate and to contrast a highly ranked hospital and its consistent performance across indicators with a low ranked hospital.To associate hospitals’ rankings with quality of care indicators, outcomes of hospitals in the highest and lowest deciles of overall nurse surveillance capacity ranking were compared. Nurse responses were aggregated to the hospital-level to create the outcome measures. Same-source bias was of concern as the nurse survey was used to measure the independent and dependent variables. Therefore, a split sample approach was used to test the robustness of the estimates. In every hospital, a random half of the nurses’ responses was aggregated for the organizational measures. The other half was aggregated for the outcome measures, and the analysis was repeated.FindingsNurse Respondent DemographicsDemographics of the nurse respondents were examined. Nearly all respondents were female (94%). The average respondent was 39 years of age. The majority of the nurses worked on a medical/surgical unit (38%); about a quarter of the respondents (24%) worked in intensive care.Nurse Surveillance Capacity DistributionsDistributions for the nurse surveillance capacity variables for the 174 study hospitals are shown. Average staffing across all hospitals was just over 5 patients per nurse. On average, one-third of the nurses within a hospital held at least a baccalaureate degree had over 13 years of experience as an RN and rated themselves as competent to proficient in their clinical expertise.A low proportion of hospitals (1 in10) had a majority of nurses who rated their practice as proficient or expert (average expertise ≥3.0).Hospitals tended to score highest on the Nursing Foundations for Quality of Care subscale of the PES-NWI; the lowest scored subscale was Staffing and Resource Adequacy.Nurse Surveillance Capacity RankingsNine percent of hospitals ranked above the 50th percentile on all nurse surveillance capacity indicators. Nurses in the highest-ranked hospitals took care of approximately two fewer patients than nurses in the lowest decile of nurse surveillance capacity.Over 40% of the nurses in the highest-ranked hospitals had a bachelor’s degree as compared to 20% of nurses in the lowest decile. Nurses in the highest decile of nurse surveillance capacity also rated their clinical expertise higher than those in the lowest grouping.Hospitals in the top decile of nurse surveillance capacity also had more years of RN experience as compared to the bottom decile. Nurses in hospitals in the top decile of nurse surveillance capacity consistently evaluated the nurse practice environment more favorably as compared to nurses in the lowest decile. The differences in means across all five subscales of the PES-NWI were sizable. The largest difference between deciles was observed in the Staffing and Resource Adequacy subscale.Figure 1 illustrates the Hospital Nurse Surveillance Capacity Profile of two hospitals chosen from the highest and lowest deciles after average ranking across indicators. The figure compares the Hospital Nurse Surveillance Capacity Profile of a hospital with consistently high rankings, theoretically demonstrating a strong capacity for nurse surveillance, with a hospital in which the capacity for surveillance may be deficient based on the proposed conceptualization. Hospital values for each of the nine indicators are displayed to provide a more meaningful comparison.The nurse surveillance capacity of hospitals was examined in this study. Nurse surveillance capacity was operationalized as a set of organizational features derived from RN characteristics and reports of the nurse practice environment collectively referred to as the Hospital Nurse Surveillance Capacity Profile.The results of this study suggest that an organization composed of well educated, expert, and experienced nurses, with adequate staffing and a supportive practice environment, promotes quality of care and prevention of two adverse events—falls with injury and nosocomial infections. The hypothesized mechanism for this association is more effective surveillance across individual nurses and overtime.The findings from this study suggest that modifying organizational features to support surveillance is a promising strategy for reducing adverse patient outcomes and improving the quality of care.The analysis confirmed that the organizational characteristics that foster nurse surveillance are associated with a better quality of care and fewer falls with injury and nosocomial infections based on self-reports from nurses. Hospitals in the highest and lowest deciles of overall nurse surveillance capacity indicators were compared relative to the quality of care and adverse events.Nurses in the highest-ranked hospitals of nurse surveillance capacity reported better quality of care and less frequent nosocomial infections and patient falls with injuries.We operationalized nurse surveillance capacity in a way that the concept could be measured. Multiple literature syntheses have demonstrated the association between nurse staffing and patient outcomes, including mortality, complications, and length of stay (Kane et al., 2007; Lang, Hodge, & Olsen, 2004; Lankshear, Sheldon, & Maynard, 2005). In a comprehensive review of the literature, Kazanjian, Green, Wong, and Reid (2005) concluded that the hospital nursing environment affects patient outcomes; however, the need for additional research was highlighted. The IOM report, Keeping Patients Safe: Transforming the Work Environment of Nurses (Page, 2004), cited all of the identified nurse surveillance capacity indicators as important factors to consider in the examination of patient care and safety.Management Applications of the ProfileIn this study, we translated the theoretical underpinnings of and organizational contributors to nurse surveillance that has been established in the literature into a measure of nurse surveillance capacity and organizational property that can be monitored and modified by nurse executives and hospital administrators.With this approach, administrators could profile their hospital’s surveillance capacity, benchmark that profile against peer hospitals with similar characteristics, and evaluate themselves over time. Benchmarking against similar hospitals with comparable macro characteristics, such as location, size, and teaching status, carries an additional advantage, as patient characteristics could be accounted for by proxy of hospital characteristics.Further, the NQF’s (2004) endorsement of the PES-NWI and the technical specifications prepared by The Joint Commission (JCAHO, 2005) allows hospitals to benchmark their institution against a broader range of hospitals. The Hospital Nurse Surveillance Capacity Profile can be used to detect weaknesses in the nurse surveillance system and highlight areas in which more attention and resource allocation are needed to ensure patient safety and quality care.The findings in this study demonstrate how the organizational capacity for nurse surveillance may be measured and evaluated. Institutions increasingly have the data to construct their own Hospital Nurse Surveillance Capacity Profiles.The Hospital Nurse Surveillance Capacity Profile could be added to a dashboard monitored by nurse executives and hospital administrators. Using the Hospital Nurse Surveillance Capacity Profile for benchmarking across like hospitals may only be possible through national databases, like the National Database of Nursing Quality Indicators (NDNQI), or health care systems that have multiple facilities, where data can be collected from and aggregated for a large number of facilities. For other facilities, the concept may be feasible for internal quality improvement at the nursing unit level. In addition to comparing similar units within hospitals, individual specialty units may follow their own surveillance capacity over time.Research ConsiderationsThis study is a secondary analysis of survey data. The use of survey data to describe organizations is challenged by the issues of respondent bias, response rate, and aggregation. The strengths of this survey data included a large number of respondents and hospitals and a research design that did not permit hospitals to opt-out.A limitation was deriving the data from nurses working in a single large state. The response rate of 52% is considered good by the current standards of social survey research (Asch, Jedrziewski, & Christakis, 1997). Individual respondents were similar to the Pennsylvania nurses in the National Sample Survey of Registered Nurses (Aiken, Clarke, Sloane, Sochalski, et al., 2002; U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, 2000). The survey data were compared previously with American Hospital Association (AHA) annual survey data and the findings revealed that the number of responding nurses from each hospital was directly proportional to the number of RNs in each hospital as reported by AHA (Aiken et al., 2003).The amount of same-source bias was likely to be reduced by the aggregation of our independent and dependent variables to the hospital-level (Rousseau, 1985; Verran et al., 1995). The effect sizes were equivalent in the split sample approach; therefore, the amount of same-source bias was negligible. Nurse informants responding to Likert-type scale items provided the outcomes in this study. The three outcome measures demonstrated reliable estimates using standard criteria. Recall bias is also a threat to the accuracy of these reports. However, nurses’ retrospective reports of adverse events have been substantiated by adverse event data collected prospectively (Aiken, Sloane, & Klocinski, 1997; Gerolamo, 2006).Although these data were collected in 1999, the concepts discussed in this study remain salient to current hospital executives (Donaldson, Brown, Aydin, Bolton, & Rutledge, 2005). Several study measures have been disseminated widely since 1999 through endorsement by the NQF (2004), the development of technical specifications byThe JCAHO (2005), and by inclusion in the annual RN Survey of the American Nurses Association-sponsored NDNQI, which was conducted in over 500 hospitals in 2007 (NDNQI, 2006; 2008).Future ResearchMethods to directly measure nurse surveillance present a challenge to the field. Nursing intervention classification systems’ measurement of nurse surveillance has been useful to begin to understand the process. However, these systems lack the specificity needed to construct a measure that accounts for each component of the surveillance process, and the organizational context in which nurses perform surveillance.To ensure the generalizability of these findings an important next research step would be to replicate and improve upon the current study using new data sets. Research on nurse surveillance capacity could expand to incorporate and evaluate patient characteristics and outcomes. By contrast to nurse reports, objective patient outcome data, including mortality, require patient-level risk-adjustment. To date, well-developed risk adjustment models exit only for surgical patient mortality using solely administrative data. Although efforts to risk-adjust mortality for medical and other patient populations have been made (Escobar, Greene, Scheirer, Gardner, Draper, et al., 2008; Tourangeau & Tu, 2003), these models need further development and refinement.Nurse surveillance capacity may be associated with other hospital structural characteristics, such as size, teaching status, and location. Opportunities to enhance nurse surveillance capacity, however, may be limited by hospital resource constraints.Additional research may reveal how units might be staffed to offset surveillance capacity inadequacies, such as insufficient RN staffing, few nurses with baccalaureate degrees, lower expertise, many new graduates, or unfavorable environments.We plan to examine this question in future work. The influence of physician workforce characteristics and technology status on the quality of surveillance will be important to consider. Moreover, information technology is a developing area, and its effectiveness on nurse surveillance remains unclear (Koppel et al., 2005).
Are there any legitimate cases of multiple personality disorder?
1968 Diagnostic and Statistical Manual (DSM-II), Multiple Personality Disorder was called hysterical neurosis, dissociative type and was defined as an alteration to consciousness and identity.In 1980, the DSM-III was published and the term "dissociative" was first introduced as a class of disorders.Separating Fact from Fiction: An Empirical Examination of Six Myths ...https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4959824/Published 2013, Dissociative identity disorder (DID) is defined in the fifth edition of the DSM ... The first published cases are those of Jeanne Fery, reported in 1586, .... in which DID or multiple personality disorder (MPD) had been diagnosed.Understanding Multiple Personality Disordershttps://www.nurseslearning.com/courses/nrp/NRP-1618/Section1/index.htm“Introduction. Since the first exploration of the phenomenon of Multiple Personality Disorder some hundred years ago, the diagnosis has been the recipient of much confusion and skepticism. Because its presentation can be so dramatic and the precipitating trauma so humanly unacceptable, it was passed off as the hysterical behavior of overwrought or spoiled women. However, with the attention in recent years to the issue of child abuse, Multiple Personality Disorder has gained acceptance as a valid psychiatric diagnosis. Once considered rare, the reported incidence has increased steadily since 1980. It occurs in 1.2% of the general psychiatric population (Steele, 1989 (making it about as common as schizophrenia).Dissociative Disorders (DD), specifically Multiple Personality Disorder (MPD), have received much attention in the past decade, though they are not new phenomena. In fact, these disorders were among the first psychiatric conditions to be scientifically investigated by the nineteenth-century pioneers of psychiatric medicine (Putnam, 1991). However, in the twentieth century, the work of such pioneers was largely set aside and forgotten as Freud introduced his psychoanalytic model which substituted the idea of repression for dissociation in dynamic formulations.MPD remains highly controversial among psychiatric professionals. The reality of the disorder is often challenged. Putnam feels “this distorts the scientific process and places an extra burden of proof on MPD that is not demanded of other psychiatric disorders.” MPD and DD have met all the requirements expected of other psychiatric diagnoses, and Putnam maintains that “by this standard, MPD and the dissociative disorders are as “real” as any other psychiatric condition.”Based on my research and having assisted many people with psychogenic amnesia and Dissociative Idenity I know these coping strategies are caused by profound, severe and relentless mental/emotional, physical and/or sexual abuse. The following sources might be of interest.1. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 5th ed Arlington, VA: APA, 2013. [Google Scholar]2. Putnam FW. Dissociation in children and adolescents: a developmental perspective. New York: Guilford, 1997. [Google Scholar]3. Simeon D, Loewenstein RJ. Dissociative disorders. In: Sadock BJ, Sadock VA, Ruiz P, editors. , eds. Kaplan and Sadock’s comprehensive textbook of psychiatry. 9th ed Philadelphia: Lippincott Williams & Wilkens, 2009;1965–2026. [Google Scholar]4. Putnam FW, Guroff JJ, Silberman EK, Barban L, Post RM. The clinical phenomenology of multiple personality disorder: review of 100 recent cases. J Clin Psychiatry 1986;47:285–93. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]5. Sar V. The many faces of dissociation: opportunities for innovative research in psychiatry. Clin Psychopharmacol Neurosci 2014;12:171–9. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]6. Herman JL. Trauma and recovery. New York: Basic, 1992. [Google Scholar]7. Rodewald F, Wilhelm-Gößling C, Emrich HM, Reddemann L, Gast U. Axis-I comorbidity in female patients with dissociative identity disorder and dissociative identity disorder not otherwise specified. J Nerv Ment Dis 2011;199:122–31. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]8. Ross CA, Miller SD, Reagor P, Bjornson L, Fraser GA, Anderson G. Schneiderian symptoms in multiple personality disorder and schizophrenia. Compr Psychiatry 1990;31:111–8. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]9. Ellason JW, Ross CA, Fuchs DL. Lifetime Axis I and II comorbidity and childhood trauma history in dissociative identity disorder. Psychiatry 1996;59:255–66. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]10. Kluft RP. The confirmation and disconfirmation of memories of abuse in DID patients: a naturalistic clinical study. Dissociation 1995;8:253–8. [Google Scholar]11. Lewis DO, Yeager CA, Swica Y, Pincus JH, Lewis M. Objective documentation of child abuse and dissociation in 12 murderers with dissociative identity disorder. Am J Psychiatry 1997;154:1703–10. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]12. Middleton W, Butler J. Dissociative identity disorder: an Australian series. Aust N Z J Psychiatry 1998;32:794–804. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]13. Swica Y, Lewis DO, Lewis M. Child abuse and dissociative identity disorder/multiple personality disorder: the documentation of childhood maltreatment and the corroboration of symptoms. Child Adolesc Psychiatr Clin N Am 1996;5:431–47. [Google Scholar]14. Dorahy MJ, Brand BL, Şar V, et al. Dissociative identity disorder: an empirical overview. Aust N Z J Psychiatry 2014;48:402–17. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]15. Carlson ET. The history of multiple personality in the United States: I. The beginnings. Am J Psychiatry 1981;138:666–8. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]16. Ellenberger HF. The discovery of the unconscious: the history and evolution of dynamic psychiatry. New York: Basic, 1970. [Google Scholar]17. Loewenstein RJ. Anna O: reformulation as a case of multiple personality disorder. In: Goodwin JM, editor. , ed. Rediscovering childhood trauma: historical casebook and clinical applications. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press, 1993;139–67. [Google Scholar]18. van der Hart O, Dorahy MJ. History of the concept of dissociation. In: Dell PF, O’Neil JA, editors. eds. Dissociation and the dissociative disorders: DSM-V and beyond. New York: Routledge, 2009:3–26. [Google Scholar]19. Sidis B, Goodhart SP. Multiple personality: an experimental investigation into the nature of human individuality. New York: D. Appleton, 1905. [Google Scholar]20. van der Hart O, Lierens R, Goodwin J. Jeanne Fery. A sixteenth-century case of dissociative identity disorder. J Psychohist 1996;24:18–35. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]21. Gmelin E. Materialen für die Anthropologie. Tübingen, Germany: Cotta, 1791. [Google Scholar]22. Guillain G. J-M. Charcot, 1825–1893: his life—his work. New York: Hoeber, 1959. [Google Scholar]23. Herman JL. Complex PTSD: a syndrome in survivors of prolonged and repeated trauma. In: Everly GS Jr, Lating JM, editors. , eds. Psychotraumatology: key papers and core concepts in post-traumatic stress. New York: Plenum, 1995;87–100. [Google Scholar]24. Chu JA. Rebuilding shattered lives: treating complex PTSD and dissociative disorders 2nd ed. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley, 2011. [Google Scholar]25. Rosenbaum M. The role of the term schizophrenia in the decline of diagnoses of multiple personality. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1980;37:1383–5. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]26. Kluft RP. First-rank symptoms as a diagnostic clue to multiple personality disorder. Am J Psychiatry 1987;144:293–8. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]27. Ross C. Dissociation in classical texts on schizophrenia. Psychosis 2014;6:342–54. [Google Scholar]28. Bleuler E. Dementia praecox or the group of schizophrenias. Oxford: International Universities, 1950. [Google Scholar]29. Dorahy MJ, van der Hart O, Middleton W. The history of early life trauma and abuse from the 1850s to the current time: how the past influences the present. In: Lanius R, Vermetten E, Pain C, editors. eds. The hidden epidemic: the impact of early life trauma on health and disease. New York: Cambridge University Press, 2010;3–12. [Google Scholar]30. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 3rd ed Washington, DC: APA, 1980. [Google Scholar]31. Tutkun H, Yargic LI, Sar V. Dissociative identity disorder presenting as hysterical psychosis. Dissociation 1996;9:244–52. [Google Scholar]32. Erikson EH. Childhood and society. New York: Norton, 1964. [Google Scholar]33. Sar V. The scope of dissociative disorders: an international perspective. Psychiatr Clin North Am 2006;29:227–44. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]34. Dalenberg C, Loewenstein R, Spiegel D, et al. Scientific study of the dissociative disorders. Psychother Psychosom 2007;76:400–1. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]35. Stein DJ, Koenen KC, Friedman MJ, et al. Dissociation in posttraumatic stress disorder: evidence from the World Mental Health Surveys. Biol Psychiatry 2013;73:302–12. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]36. Brand BL, Lanius R, Vermetten E, Loewenstein RJ, Spiegel D. Where are we going? An update on assessment, treatment, and neurobiological research in dissociative disorders as we move toward the DSM-5. J Trauma Dissociation 2012;13:9–31. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]37. Foote B, Smolin Y, Kaplan M, Legatt ME, Lipschitz D. Prevalence of dissociative disorders in psychiatric outpatients. Am J Psychiatry 2006;163:623–9. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]38. Friedl MC, Draijer N. Dissociative disorders in Dutch psychiatric inpatients. Am J Psychiatry 2000;157:1012–3. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]39. Gast U, Rodewald F, Nickel V, Emrich HM. Prevalence of dissociative disorders among psychiatric inpatients in a German university clinic. J Nerv Ment Dis 2001;189:249–57. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]40. Horen SA, Leichner PP, Lawson JS. Prevalence of dissociative symptoms and disorders in an adult psychiatric inpatient population in Canada. Can J Psychiatry 1995;40:185–91. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]41. Latz TT, Kramer SI, Hughes DL. Multiple personality disorder among female inpatients in a state hospital. Am J Psychiatry 1995;152:1343–8. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]42. Lewis-Fernández R, Martínez-Taboas A, Sar V, Patel S, Boatin A. The cross-cultural assessment of dissociation. In: Wilson JP, So-Kum Tang CC, editors. , eds. Cross-cultural assessment of psychological trauma and PTSD. New York: Springer, 2007;279–317. [Google Scholar]43. Lussier RG, Steiner J, Grey A, Hansen C. Prevalence of dissociative disorders in an acute care day hospital population. Psychiatr Serv 1997;48:244–6. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]44. Ross CA, Anderson G, Fleisher WP, Norton GR. The frequency of multiple personality disorder among psychiatric inpatients. Am J Psychiatry 1991;148:1717–20. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]45. Saxe GN, Van der Kolk BA, Berkowitz R, et al. Dissociative disorders in psychiatric inpatients. Am J Psychiatry 1993;150:1037–42. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]46. Spiegel D, Loewenstein RJ, Lewis‐Fernández R, et al. Dissociative disorders in DSM‐5. Depress Anxiety 2011;28:E17–45. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]47. Brand BL, Loewenstein RJ, Spiegel D. Dispelling myths about dissociative identity disorder treatment: an empirically based approach. Psychiatry 2014;77:169–89. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]48. Dalenberg CJ, Brand BL, Gleaves DH, et al. Evaluation of the evidence for the trauma and fantasy models of dissociation. Psychol Bull 2012;138:550–88. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]49. Dalenberg CJ, Brand BL, Loewenstein RJ, et al. Reality versus fantasy: reply to Lynn et al. (2014). Psychol Bull 2014;140:911–20. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]50. Paris J. The rise and fall of dissociative identity disorder. J Nerv Ment Dis 2012;200:1076–9. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]51. Pope HG, Jr, Barry S, Bodkin A, Hudson JI. Tracking scientific interest in the dissociative disorders: a study of scientific publication output 1984–2003. Psychother Psychosom 2006;75:19–24. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]52. McHugh P. Do fads ever die? J Nerv Ment Dis 2013;201:357–8. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]53. Definition of FAD54. Steinberg M. Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Dissociative Disorders (SCID-D). Rev. ed Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press, 1994. [Google Scholar]55. Gleaves DH, May MC, Cardeña E. An examination of the diagnostic validity of dissociative identity disorder. Clin Psychol Rev 2001;21:577–608. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]56. Ross CA, Heber S, Norton GR, Anderson D. The Dissociative Disorders Interview Schedule: a structured interview. Dissociation 1989;2:169–89. [Google Scholar]57. Loewenstein RJ. An office mental status examination for complex chronic dissociative symptoms and multiple personality disorder. Psychiatr Clin North Am 1991;14:567–604. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]58. Brand BL, Armstrong JG, Loewenstein RJ, McNary SW. Personality differences on the Rorschach of dissociative identity disorder, borderline personality disorder, and psychotic inpatients. Psychol Trauma 2009;1:188–205. [Google Scholar]59. Brand B, Loewenstein RJ. Dissociative disorders: an overview of assessment, phenomenology and treatment. Psychiatr Times 2010. (Oct);27:62–9. [Google Scholar]60. Brand BL, Chasson GS. Distinguishing simulated from genuine dissociative identity disorder on the MMPI-2. Psychol Trauma 2015;7:93–101. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]61. Brand BL, Tursich M, Tzall D, Loewenstein RJ. Utility of the SIRS-2 in distinguishing genuine from simulated dissociative identity disorder. Psychol Trauma 2014;6:308–17. [Google Scholar]62. Reinders AA, Nijenhuis ER, Quak J, et al. Psychobiological characteristics of dissociative identity disorder: a symptom provocation study. Biol Psychiatry 2006;60:730–40. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]63. Reinders AATS, Willemsen ATM, Vos HPJ, den Boer JA, Nijenhuis ERS. Fact or factitious? A psychobiological study of authentic and simulated dissociative identity states. PLoS One 2012;7:e39279. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]64. Brand BL, Classen CC, McNary SW, Zaveri P. A review of dissociative disorders treatment studies. J Nerv Ment Dis 2009;197:646–54. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]65. Brand BL, Myrick AC, Loewenstein RJ, et al. A survey of practices and recommended treatment interventions among expert therapists treating patients with dissociative identity disorder and dissociative disorder not otherwise specified. Psychol Trauma 2012;4:490–500. [Google Scholar]66. International Society for the Study of Trauma and Dissociation. Guidelines for treating dissociative identity disorder in adults, third revision: summary version. J Trauma Dissociation 2011;12:188–212. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]67. Adityanjee Raju GS, Khandelwal SK. Current status of multiple personality disorder in India. Am J Psychiatry 1989;146:1607–10. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]68. Lynn SJ, Fassler O, Knox JA, Lilienfeld SO. Dissociation and dissociative identity disorder: treatment guidelines and cautions. In: Fisher JE, O’Donohue WT, editors. , eds. Practitioner’s guide to evidence-based psychotherapy. New York: Springer, 2006. [Google Scholar]69. Lynn SJ, Lilienfeld SO, Merckelbach H, Giesbrecht T, van der Kloet D. Dissociation and dissociative disorders: challenging conventional wisdom. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 2012;21:48–53. [Google Scholar]70. Spanos NP. Multiple identity enactments and multiple personality disorder: a sociocognitive perspective. Psychol Bull 1994;116:143–65. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]71. Modestin J, Ebner G, Junghan M, Erni T. Dissociative experiences and dissociative disorders in acute psychiatric inpatients. Compr Psychiatry 1996;37:355–61. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]72. Tutkun H, Sar V, Yargiç LI, Ozpulat T, Yanik M, Kiziltan E. Frequency of dissociative disorders among psychiatric inpatients in a Turkish university clinic. Am J Psychiatry 1998;155:800–5. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]73. Ginzburg K, Somer E, Tamarkin G, Kramer L. Clandestine psychopathology: unrecognized dissociative disorders in inpatient psychiatry. J Nerv Ment Dis 2010;198:378–81. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]74. Sar V, Tutkun H, Alyanak B, Bakim B, Baral I. Frequency of dissociative disorders among psychiatric outpatients in Turkey. Compr Psychiatry 2000;41:216–22. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]75. Sar V, Kundakci T, Kiziltan E, et al. The Axis-I dissociative disorder comorbidity of borderline personality disorder among psychiatric outpatients. J Trauma Dissociation 2003;4:119–36. [Google Scholar]76. Ross CA. Epidemiology of multiple personality disorder and dissociation. Psychiatr Clin North Am 1991;14:503–17. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]77. Johnson JG, Cohen P, Kasen S, Brook JS. Dissociative disorders among adults in the community, impaired functioning, and Axis I and II comorbidity. J Psychiatr Res 2006;40:131–40. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]78. Şar V, Akyüz G, Doğan O. Prevalence of dissociative disorders among women in the general population. Psychiatry Res 2007;149:169–76. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]79. Tamar-Gurol D, Sar V, Karadag F, Evren C, Karagoz M. Childhood emotional abuse, dissociation, and suicidality among patients with drug dependency in Turkey. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 2008;62:540–7. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]80. Şar V. Epidemiology of dissociative disorders: an overview. Epidemiol Res Int 2011;2011:404538. [Google Scholar]81. Brand B, Classen C, Lanins R, et al. A naturalistic study of dissociative identity disorder and dissociative disorder not otherwise specified patients treated by community clinicians. Psychol Trauma 2009;1:153–71. [Google Scholar]82. Boysen GA, VanBergen A. A review of published research on adult dissociative identity disorder: 2000–2010. J Nerv Ment Dis 2013;201:5–11. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]83. Lilienfeld SO, Kirsch I, Sarbin TR, et al. Dissociative identity disorder and the sociocognitive model: recalling the lessons of the past. Psychol Bull 1999;125:507–23. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]84. Conklin CZ, Westen D. Borderline personality disorder in clinical practice. Am J Psychiatry 2005;162:867–75. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]85. Leonard D, Brann S, Tiller J. Dissociative disorders: pathways to diagnosis, clinician attitudes and their impact. Aust N Z J Psychiatry 2005;39:940–6. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]86. Loewenstein RJ, Putnam FW. The clinical phenomenology of males with MPD: a report of 21 cases. Dissociation 1990;3:135–43. [Google Scholar]87. Martínez-Taboas A. Multiple personality in Puerto Rico: analysis of fifteen cases. Dissociation 1991;4:189–92. [Google Scholar]88. Ross CA, Miller SD, Reagor P, Bjornson L, Fraser GA, Anderson G. Structured interview data on 102 cases of multiple personality disorder from four centers. Am J Psychiatry 1990;147:596–601. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]89. Middleton W. Dissociative disorders: a personal ‘work in progress.’ Australas Psychiatry 2004;12:245–52. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]90. Xiao Z, Yan H, Wang Z, et al. Trauma and dissociation in China. Am J Psychiatry 2006;163:1388–91. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]91. Mueller C, Moergeli H, Assaloni H, Schneider R, Rufer M. Dissociative disorders among chronic and severely impaired psychiatric outpatients. Psychopathology 2007;40:470–1. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]92. Ferdinand RF, van der Reijden M, Verhulst FC, Nienhuis FJ, Giel R. Assessment of the prevalence of psychiatric disorder in young adults. Br J Psychiatry 1995;166:480–8. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]93. Lieb R, Pfister H, Mastaler M, Wittchen H-U. Somatoform syndromes and disorders in a representative population sample of adolescents and young adults: prevalence, comorbidity and impairments. Acta Psychiatr Scand 2000;101:194–208. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]94. Mendez N, Martinez-Taboas A, Pedrosa O. Experiences, beliefs and attitudes of Puerto Rican psychologists toward dissociative identity disorder. Cienc Conducta 2000;15:69–84. [Google Scholar]95. Perniciaro LA. The influence of skepticism and clinical experience on the detection of dissociative identity disorder by mental health clinicians. Newton, MA: Massachusetts School of Professional Psychology, 2014. [Google Scholar]96. Dorahy MJ, Lewis CA, Mulholland C. The detection of dissociative identity disorder by Northern Irish clinical psychologists and psychiatrists: a clinical vignettes study. J Trauma Dissociation 2005;6:39–50. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]97. Beidel D, Bulik C, Stanley M. Abnormal psychology. 3rd ed Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson Education, 2014. [Google Scholar]98. Butcher J, Mineka S, Hooley J. Abnormal psychology. 15th ed Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson Education, 2013. [Google Scholar]99. Oltmanns T, Emery R. Abnormal psychology. 7th ed Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson Education, 2012. [Google Scholar]100. Cardeña E, van Duijl M, Weiner LA, Terhune DB. Possession/trance phenomena. In: Dell PF, O’Neil JA, editors. , eds. Dissociation and the dissociative disorders: DSM-V and beyond. New York: Routledge, 2009;171–81. [Google Scholar]101. Ross CA. Possession experiences in dissociative identity disorder: a preliminary study. J Trauma Dissociation 2011;12:393–400. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]102. Sar V, Alioğlu F, Akyüz G. Experiences of possession and paranormal phenomena among women in the general population: are they related to traumatic stress and dissociation? J Trauma Dissociation 2014;15:303–18. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]103. Kihlstrom JR. Dissociative disorders. Annu Rev Clin Psychol 2005;1:227–53. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]104. Lynn SJ, Lilienfeld SO, Merckelbach H, et al. The trauma model of dissociation: inconvenient truths and stubborn fictions. Comment on Dalenberg et al. (2012). Psychol Bull 2014;140:896–910. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]105. McHugh P. Resolved: multiple personality disorder is an individually and socially created artifact: affirmative. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1995;34:957–9. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]106. Piper A, Merskey H. The persistence of folly: a critical examination of dissociative identity disorder. Part I. The excesses of an improbable concept. Can J Psychiatry 2004;49:592–600. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]107. Spanos NP, Burgess C. Hypnosis and multiple personality disorder: a sociocognitive perspective. In: Lynn SJ, Rhue JW, editors. , eds. Dissociation: clinical and theoretical perspectives. New York: Guilford, 1994;136–55. [Google Scholar]108. Brown D, Frischholz EJ, Scheflin AW. Iatrogenic dissociative identity disorder—an evaluation of the scientific evidence. J Psychiatry Law 1999;27:549–637. [Google Scholar]109. Gleaves DH. The sociocognitive model of dissociative identity disorder: a reexamination of the evidence. Psychol Bull 1996;120:42–59. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]110. Gleaves DH, Hernandez E, Warner MS. The etiology of dissociative identity disorder: reply to Gee, Allen and Powell (2003). Prof Psychol Res Pr 2003;34:116–8. [Google Scholar]111. Kihlstrom JF, Glisky ML, Angiulo MJ. Dissociative tendencies and dissociative disorders. J Abnorm Psychol 1994;103:117–24. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]112. Spanos NP, Weekes JR, Menary E, Bertrand LD. Hypnotic interview and age regression procedures in the elicitation of multiple personality symptoms: a simulation study. Psychiatry 1986;49:298–311. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]113. Butcher JN, Graham JR, Ben-Porath YS, Tellegen A, Dahlstrom WG. Manual for the administration and scoring of the MMPI-2. Minneapolis: Minnesota University Press, 2001. [Google Scholar]114. Brand BL, Chasson GS, Polermo CA, Donato FM, Rhodes KP, Voorhees EF. Truth is in the details: a comparison of MMPI-2 item endorsements by patients with dissociative identity disorder patients versus simulators. J Am Acad Psychiatry Law (forthcoming) . [Google Scholar]115. Rogers R, Sewell KW, Gillard ND. Structured Interview of Reported Symptoms-2 (SIRS-2) and professional manual. Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources, 2010. [Google Scholar]116. Brand BL, McNary SW, Loewenstein RJ, Kolos AC, Barr SR. Assessment of genuine and simulated dissociative identity disorder on the structured interview of reported symptoms. J Trauma Dissociation 2006;7:63–85. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]117. Yu J, Ross CA, Keyes BB, et al. Dissociative disorders among Chinese inpatients diagnosed with schizophrenia. J Trauma Dissociation 2010;11:358–72. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]118. Akyüz G, Doğan O, Sar V, Yargiç LI, Tutkun H. Frequency of dissociative identity disorder in the general population in Turkey. Compr Psychiatry 1999;40:151–9. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]119. Gleaves DH, Hernandez E, Warner MS. Corroborating premorbid dissociative symptomatology in dissociative identity disorder. Prof Psychol Res Pr 1999;30:341–5. [Google Scholar]120. Chu JA, Frey LM, Ganzel BL, Matthews JA. Memories of childhood abuse: dissociation, amnesia, and corroboration. Am J Psychiatry 1999;156:749–55. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]121. Coons PM. Confirmation of childhood abuse in child and adolescent cases of multiple personality disorder and dissociative disorder not otherwise specified. J Nerv Ment Dis 1994;182:461–4. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]122. Myrick AC, Chasson GS, Lanius R, Leventhal B, Brand BL. Treatment of complex dissociative disorders: a comparison of interventions reported by community therapists versus those recommended by experts. J Trauma Dissociation 2015;16:51–67. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]123. Myrick AC, Brand BL, Putnam FW. For better or worse: the role of revictimization and stress in the course of treatment for dissociative disorders. J Trauma Dissociation 2013;14:375–89. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]124. Lauer J, Black DW, Keen P. Multiple personality disorder and borderline personality disorder: distinct entities of variations on a common theme? Ann Clin Psychiatry 1993;5:129–34. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]125. Dell P, Laddis A. Is borderline personality disorder a dissociative disorder? Paper presented at the European Society for Trauma and Dissociation conference, Belfast, April; 2010. [Google Scholar]126. Korzekwa MI, Dell PF, Links PS, Thabane L, Fougere P. Dissociation in borderline personality disorder: a detailed look. J Trauma Dissociation 2009;10:346–67. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]127. Kemp K, Gilbertson AD, Torem MS. The differential diagnosis of multiple personality disorder from borderline personality disorder. Dissociation 1988;1:41–6. [Google Scholar]128. Boon S, Draijer N. The differentiation of patients with MPD or DDNOS from patients with a cluster B personality disorder. Dissociation 1993;6:126–35. [Google Scholar]129. Hall TJ. Rorschach indices of dissociation across multiple diagnostic groups. Ann Arbor, MI: ProQuest Dissertations, 2002. [Google Scholar]130. Sar V, Akyuz G, Kugu N, Ozturk E, Ertem-Vehid H. Axis I dissociative disorder comorbidity in borderline personality disorder and reports of childhood trauma. J Clin Psychiatry 2006;67:1583–90. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]131. Ross CA. Borderline. personality disorder and dissociation. J Trauma Dissociation 2007;8:71–80. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]132. Dell PF. Axis II pathology in outpatients with dissociative identity disorder. J Nerv Ment Dis 1998;186:352–6. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]133. Ellason JW, Ross CA, Fuchs DL. Assessment of dissociative identity disorder with the Millon Clinical Multiaxial Inventory–II. Psychol Rep 1995;76:895–905. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]134. Ross CA, Ferrell L, Schroeder E. Co-occurrence of dissociative identity disorder and borderline personality disorder. J Trauma Dissociation 2014;15:79–90. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]135. Sar V, Alioğlu F, Akyuz G, Karabulut S. Dissociative amnesia in dissociative disorders and borderline personality disorder: self-rating assessment in a college population. J Trauma Dissociation 2014;15:477–93. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]136. Schmahl C, Bremner JD. Neuroimaging in borderline personality disorder. J Psychiatr Res 2006;40:419–27. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]137. Schlumpf YR, Nijenhuis ERS, Chalavi S, et al. Dissociative part-dependent biopsychosocial reactions to backward masked angry and neutral faces: an fMRI study of dissociative identity disorder. Neuroimage Clin 2013;3:54–64. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]138. Schlumpf YR, Reinders AA, Nijenhuis ER, Luechinger R, van Osch MJ, Jancke L. Dissociative part-dependent resting-state activity in dissociative identity disorder: a controlled fMRI perfusion study. PLoS One 2014;9:e98795. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]139. Sar V, Unal SN, Ozturk E. Frontal and occipital perfusion changes in dissociative identity disorder. Psychiatry Res 2007;156:217–23. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]140. Ellason JW, Ross CA. Two-year follow-up of inpatients with dissociative identity disorder. Am J Psychiatry 1997;154:832–9. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]141. Battle CL, Shea MT, Johnson DM, et al. Childhood maltreatment associated with adult personality disorders: findings from the collaborative longitudinal personality disorders study. J Pers Disord 2004;18:193–211. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]142. Classen CC, Pain C, Field NP, Woods P. Posttraumatic personality disorder: a reformulation of complex posttraumatic stress disorder and borderline personality disorder. Psychiatr Clin North Am 2006;29:87–112. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]143. Harari D, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, van Ijzendoorn MJ. Attachment, disorganization, and dissociation. In: Vermetten E, Dorahy M, Spiegel D, editors. , eds. Traumatic dissociation: neurobiology and treatment. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Publishing, 2007;31–54. [Google Scholar]144. Levy KN. The implications of attachment theory and research for understanding borderline personality disorder. Dev Psychopathol 2005;17:959–86. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]145. Becker-Blease KA, Deater-Deckard K, Eley T, Freyd JJ, Stevenson J, Plomin R. A genetic analysis of individual differences in dissociative behaviors in childhood and adolescence. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2004;45:522–32. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]146. Jang KL, Paris J, Zweig-Frank H, Livesley WJ. Twin study of dissociative experience. J Nerv Ment Dis 1998;186:345–51. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]147. Torgersen S, Lygren S, Øien PA, et al. A twin study of personality disorders. Compr Psychiatry 2000;41:416–25. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]148. Waller NG, Ross CA. The prevalence and biometric structure of pathological dissociation in the general population: taxometric and behavior genetic findings. J Abnorm Psychol 1997;106:499–510. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]149. Zanarini MC, Frankenburg FR, Yong L, et al. Borderline psychopathology in the first-degree relatives of borderline and Axis II comparison probands. J Pers Disord 2004;18:449–7. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]150. Sar V, Ross C. Dissociative disorders as a confounding factor in psychiatric research. Psychiatr Clin North Am 2006;29:129. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]151. Gee T, Allen K, Powell RA. Questioning premorbid dissociative symptomatology in dissociative identity disorder: comment on Gleaves, Hernandez and Warner (1999). Prof Psychol Res Pr 2003;34:114–6. [Google Scholar]152. Lilienfeld SO. Psychological treatments that cause harm. Perspect Psychol Sci 2007;2:53–70. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]153. Lambert K, Lilienfeld SO. Brain stains. Sci Am Mind 2007;18:46. [Google Scholar]154. Ellason JW, Ross CA. Millon Clinical Multiaxial Inventory–II. Follow-up of patients with dissociative identity disorder. Psychol Rep 1996;78:707–16. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]155. Kluft RP. Treatment of multiple personality disorder. A study of 33 cases. Psychiatr Clin North Am 1984;7:9–29. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]156. Ross CA, Haley C. Acute stabilization and three-month follow-up in a trauma program. J Trauma Dissociation 2004;5:103–12. [Google Scholar]157. Coons PM, Bowman ES. Ten-year follow-up study of patients with dissociative identity disorder. J Trauma Dissociation 2001;2:73–89. [Google Scholar]158. Coons PM. Treatment progress in 20 patients with multiple personality disorder. J Nerv Ment Dis 1986;174:715–21. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]159. Brand BL, McNary SW, Myrick AC, et al. A longitudinal naturalistic study of patients with dissociative disorders treated by community clinicians. Psychol Trauma 2013;5:301–8. [Google Scholar]160. Cronin E, Brand BL, Mattanah JF. The impact of the therapeutic alliance on treatment outcome in patients with dissociative disorders. Eur J Psychotraumatology 2014;5:1–9. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]161. Jepsen EKK, Langeland W, Sexton H, Heir T. Inpatient treatment for early sexually abused adults: a naturalistic 12-month follow-up study. Psychol Trauma 2014;6:142–51. [Google Scholar]162. Foote B, Smolin Y, Neft DI, Lipschitz D. Dissociative disorders and suicidality in psychiatric outpatients. J Nerv Ment Dis 2008;196:29–36. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]163. Mueller-Pfeiffer C, Rufibach K, Perron N, et al. Global functioning and disability in dissociative disorders. Psychiatry Res 2012;200:475–81. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]164. Loewenstein RJ. Diagnosis, epidemiology, clinical course, treatment, and cost effectiveness of treatment for dissociative disorders and MPD: report submitted to the Clinton Administration Task Force on Health Care Financing Reform. Dissociation 1994;7:3–11. [Google Scholar]165. Ross CA, Dua V. Psychiatric health care costs of multiple personality disorder. Am J Psychother 1993;47:103–12. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]166. Lloyd M. How investing in therapeutic services provides a clinical cost saving in the long term. 2011. At 1 September 2011167. Myrick AC, Brand BL, McNary SW, et al. An exploration of young adults’ progress in treatment for dissociative disorder. J Trauma Dissociation 2012;13:582–95. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]168. Kluft RP. The older female patient with a complex chronic dissociative disorder. J Women Aging 2007;19:119–37. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]169. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition (DSM-5). Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing, 2013.170. Carlson, E. B. & Putnam, F. W. DES: Dissociative Experiences Scale II Accessed 4/20/2016.171. International Society for the Study of Trauma and Dissociative Disorders. Trauma and Dissociative Disorders FAQs Accessed 4/20/2016.172. Mental Health America. Dissociation and Dissociative Disorders Accessed 4/20/2016.173. Steinberg, M. Interviewers’ guide to the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Dissociative Disorders (SCID-D). Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing, 1994.Sunny Dawn: Thank you for inviting me to answer your question. I am here only to be truly helpful.
Can tulpamancy skills be used to help people with DID gain awareness of their alters?
A tulpa is an alter-self created in the mind, acting independently of, and parallel to your core consciousness. The alter-self is able to think, and have their own free will, emotions, and memories. In short, a tulpa is like a sentient person living in your head, separate from your core self. A tulpa is created for self-preservation during an extended period of horrific mental/emotional, physical, and/or sexual abuse.While tulpamancy skills are a good coping mechanism the amount of time your core self is excluded in day-to-day interactions is fundamentally the same. In fact, in some instances, one of the alter-self proclaims it has the right to become an alternate core self.Each alter-self needs to discover what experiences and trauma each hold and process the experience and trauma to the degree the alter-self can integrate into the core-self.When someone begins a holistic transformation/transmuting process, no matter how dire their predicament seems to be, I KNOW if she/he is WILLING to do the mental/ emotional discovery work; releasing and transforming beliefs, thoughts and feelings, anything can be transformed/transmuted. The word ‘incurable’ or ‘impossible’ only means that the particular condition, symptom or diagnosis cannot be ‘cured’ by ‘outer’ methods and that she/he needs to GO WITHIN to effect the transformation/transmuting. The condition, symptom or diagnosis came from mental/emotional distress and will go back to nothing.When beliefs, thoughts, feelings, and behavior are accessed and addressed at the unconscious, subconscious and cellular level, the 'cause' of any and all symptoms and behavior become crystal clear--it is mental/emotional, physical, and spiritual trauma/distress manifesting in the behavior and symptoms you experience.A Transformation/Transmuting process is a clear, concise, and direct method of transforming/transmuting the mental, emotional and physical symptoms that transcends traditional protocols while retaining a professional focus. Deep Healing avoids prescription and OTC drugs, body parts removed, artificial hypnotic inductions, and psychic interventions. The process ties in directly with the experiences and needs of the person. The process is down-to-earth, to-the-point, practical, fearless and with 20+ years experience and centuries of holistic health care protocol success I know there is no doubt Deep Healing is effective.1968 Diagnostic and Statistical Manual (DSM-II), Multiple Personality Disorder was called hysterical neurosis, dissociative type, and was defined as an alteration to consciousness and identity.In 1980, the DSM-III was published, and the term "dissociative" was first introduced as a class of disorders.Separating Fact from Fiction: An Empirical Examination of Six Myths ...Separating Fact from Fiction: An Empirical Examination of Six Myths About Dissociative Identity DisorderPublished 2013, Dissociative identity disorder (DID) is defined in the fifth edition of the DSM ... The first published cases are those of Jeanne Fery, reported in 1586, .... in which DID or multiple personality disorder (MPD) had been diagnosed.Understanding Multiple Personality DisordersUnderstanding Multiple Personality Disorders“Introduction. Since the first exploration of the phenomenon of Multiple Personality Disorder some hundred years ago, the diagnosis has been the recipient of much confusion and skepticism. Because its presentation can be so dramatic and the precipitating trauma so humanly unacceptable, it was passed off as the hysterical behavior of overwrought or spoiled women. However, with the attention in recent years to the issue of child abuse, Multiple Personality Disorder has gained acceptance as a valid psychiatric diagnosis. Once considered rare, the reported incidence has increased steadily since 1980. It occurs in 1.2% of the general psychiatric population (Steele, 1989 (making it about as common as schizophrenia).Dissociative Identity Disorders (DID), specifically Multiple Personality Disorder (MPD), have received much attention in the past decade, though they are not new phenomena. In fact, these disorders were among the first psychiatric conditions to be scientifically investigated by the nineteenth-century pioneers of psychiatric medicine (Putnam, 1991). However, in the twentieth century, the work of such pioneers was largely set aside and forgotten as Freud introduced his psychoanalytic model which substituted the idea of repression for dissociation in dynamic formulations.MPD remains highly controversial among psychiatric professionals. The reality of the disorder is often challenged. Putnam feels “this distorts the scientific process and places an extra burden of proof on MPD that is not demanded of other psychiatric disorders.” MPD and DD have met all the requirements expected of other psychiatric diagnoses, and Putnam maintains that “by this standard, MPD and the dissociative disorders are as “real” as any other psychiatric condition.”Based on my research and having assisted many people with psychogenic amnesia and Dissociative Identity Disorder I know these coping strategies are caused by profound, severe, and relentless mental/emotional, physical, and/or sexual abuse. The following sources might be of interest.1. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 5th ed Arlington, VA: APA, 2013. [Google Scholar]2. Putnam FW. Dissociation in children and adolescents: a developmental perspective. New York: Guilford, 1997. [Google Scholar]3. Simeon D, Loewenstein RJ. Dissociative disorders. In: Sadock BJ, Sadock VA, Ruiz P, editors. , eds. Kaplan and Sadock’s comprehensive textbook of psychiatry. 9th ed Philadelphia: Lippincott Williams & Wilkens, 2009;1965–2026. [Google Scholar]4. Putnam FW, Guroff JJ, Silberman EK, Barban L, Post RM. The clinical phenomenology of multiple personality disorder: review of 100 recent cases. J Clin Psychiatry 1986;47:285–93. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]5. Sar V. The many faces of dissociation: opportunities for innovative research in psychiatry. Clin Psychopharmacol Neurosci 2014;12:171–9. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]6. Herman JL. Trauma and recovery. New York: Basic, 1992. [Google Scholar]7. Rodewald F, Wilhelm-Gößling C, Emrich HM, Reddemann L, Gast U. Axis-I comorbidity in female patients with dissociative identity disorder and dissociative identity disorder not otherwise specified. J Nerv Ment Dis 2011;199:122–31. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]8. Ross CA, Miller SD, Reagor P, Bjornson L, Fraser GA, Anderson G. Schneiderian symptoms in multiple personality disorder and schizophrenia. Compr Psychiatry 1990;31:111–8. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]9. Ellason JW, Ross CA, Fuchs DL. Lifetime Axis I and II comorbidity and childhood trauma history in dissociative identity disorder. Psychiatry 1996;59:255–66. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]10. Kluft RP. The confirmation and disconfirmation of memories of abuse in DID patients: a naturalistic clinical study. Dissociation 1995;8:253–8. [Google Scholar]11. Lewis DO, Yeager CA, Swica Y, Pincus JH, Lewis M. Objective documentation of child abuse and dissociation in 12 murderers with a dissociative identity disorder. Am J Psychiatry 1997;154:1703–10. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]12. Middleton W, Butler J. Dissociative identity disorder: an Australian series. Aust N Z J Psychiatry 1998;32:794–804. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]13. Swica Y, Lewis DO, Lewis M. Child abuse and dissociative identity disorder/multiple personality disorder: the documentation of childhood maltreatment and the corroboration of symptoms. Child Adolesc Psychiatr Clin N Am 1996;5:431–47. [Google Scholar]14. Dorahy MJ, Brand BL, Şar V, et al. Dissociative identity disorder: an empirical overview. Aust N Z J Psychiatry 2014;48:402–17. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]15. Carlson ET. The history of multiple personalities in the United States: I. The beginnings. Am J Psychiatry 1981;138:666–8. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]16. Ellenberger HF. The discovery of the unconscious: the history and evolution of dynamic psychiatry. New York: Basic, 1970. [Google Scholar]17. Loewenstein RJ. Anna O: reformulation as a case of multiple personality disorder. In: Goodwin JM, editor. , ed. Rediscovering childhood trauma: historical casebook and clinical applications. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press, 1993;139–67. [Google Scholar]18. van der Hart O, Dorahy MJ. History of the concept of dissociation. In: Dell PF, O’Neil JA, editors. eds. Dissociation and the dissociative disorders: DSM-V and beyond. New York: Routledge, 2009:3–26. [Google Scholar]19. Sidis B, Goodhart SP. Multiple personality: an experimental investigation into the nature of human individuality. New York: D. Appleton, 1905. [Google Scholar]20. van der Hart O, Lierens R, Goodwin J. Jeanne Fery. A sixteenth-century case of dissociative identity disorder. J Psychohist 1996;24:18–35. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]21. Gmelin E. Materialen für die Anthropologie. Tübingen, Germany: Cotta, 1791. [Google Scholar]22. Guillain G. J-M. Charcot, 1825–1893: his life—his work. New York: Hoeber, 1959. [Google Scholar]23. Herman JL. Complex PTSD: a syndrome in survivors of prolonged and repeated trauma. In: Everly GS Jr, Lating JM, editors. , eds. Psychotraumatology: key papers and core concepts in post-traumatic stress. New York: Plenum, 1995;87–100. [Google Scholar]24. Chu JA. Rebuilding shattered lives: treating complex PTSD and dissociative disorders 2nd ed. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley, 2011. [Google Scholar]25. Rosenbaum M. The role of the term schizophrenia in the decline of diagnoses of multiple personality. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1980;37:1383–5. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]26. Kluft RP. First-rank symptoms as a diagnostic clue to multiple personality disorder. Am J Psychiatry 1987;144:293–8. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]27. Ross C. Dissociation in classical texts on schizophrenia. Psychosis 2014;6:342–54. [Google Scholar]28. Bleuler E. Dementia praecox or the group of schizophrenias. Oxford: International Universities, 1950. [Google Scholar]29. Dorahy MJ, van der Hart O, Middleton W. The history of early life trauma and abuse from the 1850s to the current time: how the past influences the present. In: Lanius R, Vermetten E, Pain C, editors. eds. The hidden epidemic: the impact of early life trauma on health and disease. New York: Cambridge University Press, 2010;3–12. [Google Scholar]30. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 3rd ed Washington, DC: APA, 1980. [Google Scholar]31. Tutkun H, Yargic LI, Sar V. Dissociative identity disorder presenting as hysterical psychosis. Dissociation 1996;9:244–52. [Google Scholar]32. Erikson EH. Childhood and society. New York: Norton, 1964. [Google Scholar]33. Sar V. The scope of dissociative disorders: an international perspective. Psychiatr Clin North Am 2006;29:227–44. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]34. Dalenberg C, Loewenstein R, Spiegel D, et al. Scientific study of the dissociative disorders. Psychother Psychosom 2007;76:400–1. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]35. Stein DJ, Koenen KC, Friedman MJ, et al. Dissociation in posttraumatic stress disorder: evidence from the World Mental Health Surveys. Biol Psychiatry 2013;73:302–12. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]36. Brand BL, Lanius R, Vermetten E, Loewenstein RJ, Spiegel D. Where are we going? An update on assessment, treatment, and neurobiological research in dissociative disorders as we move toward the DSM-5. J Trauma Dissociation 2012;13:9–31. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]37. Foote B, Smolin Y, Kaplan M, Legatt ME, Lipschitz D. Prevalence of dissociative disorders in psychiatric outpatients. Am J Psychiatry 2006;163:623–9. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]38. Friedl MC, Draijer N. Dissociative disorders in Dutch psychiatric inpatients. Am J Psychiatry 2000;157:1012–3. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]39. Gast U, Rodewald F, Nickel V, Emrich HM. Prevalence of dissociative disorders among psychiatric inpatients in a German university clinic. J Nerv Ment Dis 2001;189:249–57. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]40. Horen SA, Leichner PP, Lawson JS. Prevalence of dissociative symptoms and disorders in an adult psychiatric inpatient population in Canada. Can J Psychiatry 1995;40:185–91. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]41. Latz TT, Kramer SI, Hughes DL. Multiple personality disorder among female inpatients in a state hospital. Am J Psychiatry 1995;152:1343–8. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]42. Lewis-Fernández R, Martínez-Taboas A, Sar V, Patel S, Boatin A. The cross-cultural assessment of dissociation. In: Wilson JP, So-Kum Tang CC, editors. , eds. Cross-cultural assessment of psychological trauma and PTSD. New York: Springer, 2007;279–317. [Google Scholar]43. Lussier RG, Steiner J, Grey A, Hansen C. Prevalence of dissociative disorders in an acute care day hospital population. Psychiatr Serv 1997;48:244–6. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]44. Ross CA, Anderson G, Fleisher WP, Norton GR. The frequency of multiple personality disorder among psychiatric inpatients. Am J Psychiatry 1991;148:1717–20. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]45. Saxe GN, Van der Kolk BA, Berkowitz R, et al. Dissociative disorders in psychiatric inpatients. Am J Psychiatry 1993;150:1037–42. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]46. Spiegel D, Loewenstein RJ, Lewis‐Fernández R, et al. Dissociative disorders in DSM‐5. Depress Anxiety 2011;28:E17–45. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]47. Brand BL, Loewenstein RJ, Spiegel D. Dispelling myths about dissociative identity disorder treatment: an empirically based approach. Psychiatry 2014;77:169–89. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]48. Dalenberg CJ, Brand BL, Gleaves DH, et al. Evaluation of the evidence for the trauma and fantasy models of dissociation. Psychol Bull 2012;138:550–88. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]49. Dalenberg CJ, Brand BL, Loewenstein RJ, et al. Reality versus fantasy: reply to Lynn et al. (2014). Psychol Bull 2014;140:911–20. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]50. Paris J. The rise and fall of dissociative identity disorder. J Nerv Ment Dis 2012;200:1076–9. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]51. Pope HG, Jr, Barry S, Bodkin A, Hudson JI. Tracking scientific interest in the dissociative disorders: a study of scientific publication output 1984–2003. Psychother Psychosom 2006;75:19–24. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]52. McHugh P. Do fads ever die? J Nerv Ment Dis 2013;201:357–8. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]53. Definition of FAD54. Steinberg M. Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Dissociative Disorders (SCID-D). Rev. ed Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press, 1994. [Google Scholar]55. Gleaves DH, May MC, Cardeña E. An examination of the diagnostic validity of dissociative identity disorder. Clin Psychol Rev 2001;21:577–608. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]56. Ross CA, Heber S, Norton GR, Anderson D. The Dissociative Disorders Interview Schedule: a structured interview. Dissociation 1989;2:169–89. [Google Scholar]57. Loewenstein RJ. An office mental status examination for complex chronic dissociative symptoms and multiple personality disorder. Psychiatr Clin North Am 1991;14:567–604. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]58. Brand BL, Armstrong JG, Loewenstein RJ, McNary SW. Personality differences on the Rorschach of dissociative identity disorder, borderline personality disorder, and psychotic inpatients. Psychol Trauma 2009;1:188–205. [Google Scholar]59. Brand B, Loewenstein RJ. Dissociative disorders: an overview of assessment, phenomenology and treatment. Psychiatr Times 2010. (Oct);27:62–9. [Google Scholar]60. Brand BL, Chasson GS. Distinguishing simulated from genuine dissociative identity disorder on the MMPI-2. Psychol Trauma 2015;7:93–101. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]61. Brand BL, Tursich M, Tzall D, Loewenstein RJ. Utility of the SIRS-2 in distinguishing genuine from simulated dissociative identity disorder. Psychol Trauma 2014;6:308–17. [Google Scholar]62. Reinders AA, Nijenhuis ER, Quak J, et al. Psychobiological characteristics of dissociative identity disorder: a symptom provocation study. Biol Psychiatry 2006;60:730–40. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]63. Reinders AATS, Willemsen ATM, Vos HPJ, den Boer JA, Nijenhuis ERS. Fact or factitious? A psychobiological study of authentic and simulated dissociative identity states. PLoS One 2012;7:e39279. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]64. Brand BL, Classen CC, McNary SW, Zaveri P. A review of dissociative disorders treatment studies. J Nerv Ment Dis 2009;197:646–54. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]65. Brand BL, Myrick AC, Loewenstein RJ, et al. A survey of practices and recommended treatment interventions among expert therapists treating patients with dissociative identity disorder and dissociative disorder not otherwise specified. Psychol Trauma 2012;4:490–500. [Google Scholar]66. International Society for the Study of Trauma and Dissociation. Guidelines for treating dissociative identity disorder in adults, third revision: summary version. J Trauma Dissociation 2011;12:188–212. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]67. Adityanjee Raju GS, Khandelwal SK. Current status of multiple personality disorder in India. Am J Psychiatry 1989;146:1607–10. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]68. Lynn SJ, Fassler O, Knox JA, Lilienfeld SO. Dissociation and dissociative identity disorder: treatment guidelines and cautions. In: Fisher JE, O’Donohue WT, editors. , eds. Practitioner’s guide to evidence-based psychotherapy. New York: Springer, 2006. [Google Scholar]69. Lynn SJ, Lilienfeld SO, Merckelbach H, Giesbrecht T, van der Kloet D. Dissociation and dissociative disorders: challenging conventional wisdom. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 2012;21:48–53. [Google Scholar]70. Spanos NP. Multiple identity enactments and multiple personality disorder: a sociocognitive perspective. Psychol Bull 1994;116:143–65. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]71. Modestin J, Ebner G, Junghan M, Erni T. Dissociative experiences and dissociative disorders in acute psychiatric inpatients. Compr Psychiatry 1996;37:355–61. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]72. Tutkun H, Sar V, Yargiç LI, Ozpulat T, Yanik M, Kiziltan E. Frequency of dissociative disorders among psychiatric inpatients in a Turkish university clinic. Am J Psychiatry 1998;155:800–5. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]73. Ginzburg K, Somer E, Tamarkin G, Kramer L. Clandestine psychopathology: unrecognized dissociative disorders in inpatient psychiatry. J Nerv Ment Dis 2010;198:378–81. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]74. Sar V, Tutkun H, Alyanak B, Bakim B, Baral I. Frequency of dissociative disorders among psychiatric outpatients in Turkey. Compr Psychiatry 2000;41:216–22. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]75. Sar V, Kundakci T, Kiziltan E, et al. The Axis-I dissociative disorder comorbidity of borderline personality disorder among psychiatric outpatients. J Trauma Dissociation 2003;4:119–36. [Google Scholar]76. Ross CA. Epidemiology of multiple personality disorder and dissociation. Psychiatr Clin North Am 1991;14:503–17. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]77. Johnson JG, Cohen P, Kasen S, Brook JS. Dissociative disorders among adults in the community, impaired functioning, and Axis I and II comorbidity. J Psychiatr Res 2006;40:131–40. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]78. Şar V, Akyüz G, Doğan O. Prevalence of dissociative disorders among women in the general population. Psychiatry Res 2007;149:169–76. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]79. Tamar-Gurol D, Sar V, Karadag F, Evren C, Karagoz M. Childhood emotional abuse, dissociation, and suicidality among patients with drug dependency in Turkey. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 2008;62:540–7. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]80. Şar V. Epidemiology of dissociative disorders: an overview. Epidemiol Res Int 2011;2011:404538. [Google Scholar]81. Brand B, Classen C, Lanins R, et al. A naturalistic study of dissociative identity disorder and dissociative disorder not otherwise specified patients treated by community clinicians. Psychol Trauma 2009;1:153–71. [Google Scholar]82. Boysen GA, VanBergen A. A review of published research on adult dissociative identity disorder: 2000–2010. J Nerv Ment Dis 2013;201:5–11. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]83. Lilienfeld SO, Kirsch I, Sarbin TR, et al. Dissociative identity disorder and the sociocognitive model: recalling the lessons of the past. Psychol Bull 1999;125:50723. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]84. Conklin CZ, Westen D. Borderline personality disorder in clinical practice. Am J Psychiatry 2005;162:867–75. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]85. Leonard D, Brann S, Tiller J. Dissociative disorders: pathways to diagnosis, clinician attitudes and their impact. Aust N Z J Psychiatry 2005;39:940–6. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]86. Loewenstein RJ, Putnam FW. The clinical phenomenology of males with MPD: a report of 21 cases. Dissociation 1990;3:135–43. [Google Scholar]87. Martínez-Taboas A. Multiple personality in Puerto Rico: analysis of fifteen cases. Dissociation 1991;4:189–92. [Google Scholar]88. Ross CA, Miller SD, Reagor P, Bjornson L, Fraser GA, Anderson G. Structured interview data on 102 cases of multiple personality disorder from four centers. Am J Psychiatry 1990;147:596–601. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]89. Middleton W. Dissociative disorders: a personal ‘work in progress.’ Australas Psychiatry 2004;12:245–52. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]90. Psychiatry 2006;163:1388–91. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]91. Mueller C, Moergeli H, Assaloni H, Schneider R, Rufer M. Dissociative disorders among chronic and severely impaired psychiatric outpatients. Psychopathology 2007;40:470–1. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]92. Ferdinand RF, van der Reijden M, Verhulst FC, Nienhuis FJ, Giel R. Assessment of the prevalence of psychiatric disorder in young adults. Br J Psychiatry 1995;166:480–8. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]93. Lieb R, Pfister H, Mastaler M, Wittchen H-U. Somatoform syndromes and disorders in a representative population sample of adolescents and young adults: prevalence, comorbidity and impairments. Acta Psychiatr Scand 2000;101:194208. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]94. Mendez N, Martinez-Taboas A, Pedrosa O. Experiences, beliefs and attitudes of Puerto Rican psychologists toward dissociative identity disorder. Cienc Conducta 2000;15:69–84. [Google Scholar]95. Perniciaro LA. The influence of skepticism and clinical experience on the detection of dissociative identity disorder by mental health clinicians. Newton, MA: Massachusetts School of Professional Psychology, 2014. [Google Scholar]96. Dorahy MJ, Lewis CA, Mulholland C. The detection of dissociative identity disorder by Northern Irish clinical psychologists and psychiatrists: a clinical vignettes study. J Trauma Dissociation 2005;6:39–50. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]97. Beidel D, Bulik C, Stanley M. Abnormal psychology. 3rd ed Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson Education, 2014. [Google Scholar]98. Butcher J, Mineka S, Hooley J. Abnormal psychology. 15th ed Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson Education, 2013. [Google Scholar]99. Oltmanns T, Emery R. Abnormal psychology. 7th ed Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson Education, 2012. [Google Scholar]100. Cardeña E, van Duijl M, Weiner LA, Terhune DB. Possession/trance phenomena. In: Dell PF, O’Neil JA, editors. , eds. Dissociation and the dissociative disorders: DSM-V and beyond. New York: Routledge, 2009;171–81. [Google Scholar]101. Ross CA. Possession experiences in dissociative identity disorder: a preliminary study. J Trauma Dissociation 2011;12:393–400. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]102. Sar V, Alioğlu F, Akyüz G. Experiences of possession and paranormal phenomena among women in the general population: are they related to traumatic stress and dissociation? J Trauma Dissociation 2014;15:303–18. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]103. Kihlstrom JR. Dissociative disorders. Annu Rev Clin Psychol 2005;1:227–53. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]104. Lynn SJ, Lilienfeld SO, Merckelbach H, et al. The trauma model of dissociation: inconvenient truths and stubborn fictions. Comment on Dalenberg et al. (2012). Psychol Bull 2014;140:896–910. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]105. McHugh P. Resolved: multiple personality disorder is an individually and socially created artifact: affirmative. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1995;34:957–9. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]106. Piper A, Merskey H. The persistence of folly: a critical examination of dissociative identity disorder. Part I. The excesses of an improbable concept. Can J Psychiatry 2004;49:592–600. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]107. Spanos NP, Burgess C. Hypnosis and multiple personality disorder: a sociocognitive perspective. In: Lynn SJ, Rhue JW, editors. , eds. Dissociation: clinical and theoretical perspectives. New York: Guilford, 1994;136–55. [Google Scholar]108. Brown D, Frischholz EJ, Scheflin AW. Iatrogenic dissociative identity disorder—an evaluation of the scientific evidence. J Psychiatry Law 1999;27:549–637. [Google Scholar]109. Gleaves DH. The sociocognitive model of dissociative identity disorder: a reexamination of the evidence. Psychol Bull 1996;120:42–59. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]110. Gleaves DH, Hernandez E, Warner MS. The etiology of dissociative identity disorder: reply to Gee, Allen and Powell (2003). Prof Psychol Res Pr 2003;34:116–8. [Google Scholar]111. Kihlstrom JF, Glisky ML, Angiulo MJ. Dissociative tendencies and dissociative disorders. J Abnorm Psychol 1994;103:117–24. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]112. Spanos NP, Weekes JR, Menary E, Bertrand LD. Hypnotic interview and age regression procedures in the elicitation of multiple personality symptoms: a simulation study. Psychiatry 1986;49:298–311. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]113. Butcher JN, Graham JR, Ben-Porath YS, Tellegen A, Dahlstrom WG. Manual for the administration and scoring of the MMPI-2. Minneapolis: Minnesota University Press, 2001. [Google Scholar]114. Brand BL, Chasson GS, Polermo CA, Donato FM, Rhodes KP, Voorhees EF. Truth is in the details: a comparison of MMPI-2 item endorsements by patients with dissociative identity disorder patients versus simulators. J Am Acad Psychiatry Law (forthcoming) . [Google Scholar]115. Rogers R, Sewell KW, Gillard ND. Structured Interview of Reported Symptoms-2 (SIRS-2) and professional manual. Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources, 2010. [Google Scholar]116. Brand BL, McNary SW, Loewenstein RJ, Kolos AC, Barr SR. Assessment of genuine and simulated dissociative identity disorder on the structured interview of reported symptoms. J Trauma Dissociation 2006;7:63–85. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]117. Yu J, Ross CA, Keyes BB, et al. Dissociative disorders among Chinese inpatients diagnosed with schizophrenia. J Trauma Dissociation 2010;11:358–72. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]118. Akyüz G, Doğan O, Sar V, Yargiç LI, Tutkun H. Frequency of dissociative identity disorder in the general population in Turkey. Compr Psychiatry 1999;40:151–9. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]119. Gleaves DH, Hernandez E, Warner MS. Corroborating premorbid dissociative symptomatology in dissociative identity disorder. Prof Psychol Res Pr 1999;30:341–5. [Google Scholar]120. Chu JA, Frey LM, Ganzel BL, Matthews JA. Memories of childhood abuse: dissociation, amnesia, and corroboration. Am J Psychiatry 1999;156:749–55. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]121. Coons PM. Confirmation of childhood abuse in child and adolescent cases of multiple personality disorder and dissociative disorder not otherwise specified. J Nerv Ment Dis 1994;182:461–4. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]122. Myrick AC, Chasson GS, Lanius R, Leventhal B, Brand BL. Treatment of complex dissociative disorders: a comparison of interventions reported by community therapists versus those recommended by experts. J Trauma Dissociation 2015;16:51–67. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]123. Myrick AC, Brand BL, Putnam FW. For better or worse: the role of revictimization and stress in the course of treatment for dissociative disorders. J Trauma Dissociation 2013;14:375–89. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]124. Lauer J, Black DW, Keen P. Multiple personality disorder and borderline personality disorder: distinct entities of variations on a common theme? Ann Clin Psychiatry 1993;5:129–34. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]125. Dell P, Laddis A. Is borderline personality disorder a dissociative disorder? Paper presented at the European Society for Trauma and Dissociation conference, Belfast, April; 2010. [Google Scholar]126. Korzekwa MI, Dell PF, Links PS, Thabane L, Fougere P. Dissociation in borderline personality disorder: a detailed look. J Trauma Dissociation 2009;10:346–67. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]127. Kemp K, Gilbertson AD, Torem MS. The differential diagnosis of multiple personality disorder from borderline personality disorder. Dissociation 1988;1:41–6. [Google Scholar]128. Boon S, Draijer N. The differentiation of patients with MPD or DDNOS from patients with a cluster B personality disorder. Dissociation 1993;6:126–35. [Google Scholar]129. Hall TJ. Rorschach indices of dissociation across multiple diagnostic groups. Ann Arbor, MI: ProQuest Dissertations, 2002. [Google Scholar]130. Sar V, Akyuz G, Kugu N, Ozturk E, Ertem-Vehid H. Axis I dissociative disorder comorbidity in borderline personality disorder and reports of childhood trauma. J Clin Psychiatry 2006;67:1583–90. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]131. Ross CA. Borderline. personality disorder and dissociation. J Trauma Dissociation 2007;8:71–80. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]132. Dell PF. Axis II pathology in outpatients with dissociative identity disorder. J Nerv Ment Dis 1998;186:352–6. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]133. Ellason JW, Ross CA, Fuchs DL. Assessment of dissociative identity disorder with the Millon Clinical Multiaxial Inventory–II. Psychol Rep 1995;76:895–905. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]134. Ross CA, Ferrell L, Schroeder E. Co-occurrence of dissociative identity disorder and borderline personality disorder. J Trauma Dissociation 2014;15:79–90. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]135. Sar V, Alioğlu F, Akyuz G, Karabulut S. Dissociative amnesia in dissociative disorders and borderline personality disorder: self-rating assessment in a college population. J Trauma Dissociation 2014;15:477–93. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]136. Schmahl C, Bremner JD. Neuroimaging in borderline personality disorder. J Psychiatr Res 2006;40:419–27. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]137. Schlumpf YR, Nijenhuis ERS, Chalavi S, et al. Dissociative part-dependent biopsychosocial reactions to backward masked angry and neutral faces: an fMRI study of dissociative identity disorder. Neuroimage Clin 2013;3:54–64. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]138. Schlumpf YR, Reinders AA, Nijenhuis ER, Luechinger R, van Osch MJ, Jancke L. Dissociative part-dependent resting-state activity in dissociative identity disorder: a controlled fMRI perfusion study. PLoS One 2014;9:e98795. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]139. Sar V, Unal SN, Ozturk E. Frontal and occipital perfusion changes in dissociative identity disorder. Psychiatry Res 2007;156:217–23. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]140. Ellason JW, Ross CA. Two-year follow-up of inpatients with dissociative identity disorder. Am J Psychiatry 1997;154:832–9. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]141. Battle CL, Shea MT, Johnson DM, et al. Childhood maltreatment associated with adult personality disorders: findings from the collaborative longitudinal personality disorders study. J Pers Disord 2004;18:193–211. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]142. Classen CC, Pain C, Field NP, Woods P. Posttraumatic personality disorder: a reformulation of complex posttraumatic stress disorder and borderline personality disorder. Psychiatr Clin North Am 2006;29:87–112. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]143. Harari D, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, van Ijzendoorn MJ. Attachment, disorganization, and dissociation. In: Vermetten E, Dorahy M, Spiegel D, editors. , eds. Traumatic dissociation: neurobiology and treatment. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Publishing, 2007;31–54. [Google Scholar]144. Levy KN. The implications of attachment theory and research for understanding borderline personality disorder. Dev Psychopathol 2005;17:959–86. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]145. Becker-Blease KA, Deater-Deckard K, Eley T, Freyd JJ, Stevenson J, Plomin R. A genetic analysis of individual differences in dissociative behaviors in childhood and adolescence. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2004;45:522–32. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]146. Jang KL, Paris J, Zweig-Frank H, Livesley WJ. Twin study of dissociative experience. J Nerv Ment Dis 1998;186:345–51. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]147. Torgersen S, Lygren S, Øien PA, et al. A twin study of personality disorders. Compr Psychiatry 2000;41:416–25. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]148. Waller NG, Ross CA. The prevalence and biometric structure of pathological dissociation in the general population: taxometric and behavior genetic findings. J Abnorm Psychol 1997;106:499–510. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]149. Zanarini MC, Frankenburg FR, Yong L, et al. Borderline psychopathology in the first-degree relatives of borderline and Axis II comparison probands. J Pers Disord 2004;18:449–7. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]150. Sar V, Ross C. Dissociative disorders as a confounding factor in psychiatric research. Psychiatr Clin North Am 2006;29:129. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]151. Gee T, Allen K, Powell RA. Questioning premorbid dissociative symptomatology in dissociative identity disorder: comment on Gleaves, Hernandez and Warner (1999). Prof Psychol Res Pr 2003;34:114–6. [Google Scholar]152. Lilienfeld SO. Psychological treatments that cause harm. Perspect Psychol Sci 2007;2:53–70. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]153. Lambert K, Lilienfeld SO. Brain stains. Sci Am Mind 2007;18:46. [Google Scholar]154. Ellason JW, Ross CA. Millon Clinical Multiaxial Inventory–II. Follow-up of patients with dissociative identity disorder. Psychol Rep 1996;78:707–16. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]155. Kluft RP. Treatment of multiple personality disorder. A study of 33 cases. Psychiatr Clin North Am 1984;7:9–29. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]156. Ross CA, Haley C. Acute stabilization and three-month follow-up in a trauma program. J Trauma Dissociation 2004;5:103–12. [Google Scholar]157. Coons PM, Bowman ES. Ten-year follow-up study of patients with dissociative identity disorder. J Trauma Dissociation 2001;2:73–89. [Google Scholar]158. Coons PM. Treatment progress in 20 patients with multiple personality disorder. J Nerv Ment Dis 1986;174:715–21. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]159. Brand BL, McNary SW, Myrick AC, et al. A longitudinal naturalistic study of patients with dissociative disorders treated by community clinicians. Psychol Trauma 2013;5:301–8. [Google Scholar]160. Cronin E, Brand BL, Mattanah JF. The impact of the therapeutic alliance on treatment outcome in patients with dissociative disorders. Eur J Psychotraumatology 2014;5:1–9. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]161. Jepsen EKK, Langeland W, Sexton H, Heir T. Inpatient treatment for early sexually abused adults: a naturalistic 12-month follow-up study. Psychol Trauma 2014;6:142–51. [Google Scholar]162. Foote B, Smolin Y, Neft DI, Lipschitz D. Dissociative disorders and suicidality in psychiatric outpatients. J Nerv Ment Dis 2008;196:29–36. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]163. Mueller-Pfeiffer C, Rufibach K, Perron N, et al. Global functioning and disability in dissociative disorders. Psychiatry Res 2012;200:475–81. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]164. Dissociation 1994;7:3–11. [Google Scholar]165. Ross CA, Dua V. Psychiatric health care costs of multiple personality disorder. Am J Psychother 1993;47:103–12. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]166. Lloyd M. How investing in therapeutic services provides a clinical cost saving in the long term. 2011. At 1 September 2011167. Myrick AC, Brand BL, McNary SW, et al. An exploration of young adults’ progress in treatment for dissociative disorder. J Trauma Dissociation 2012;13:582–95. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]168. Kluft RP. The older female patient with a complex chronic dissociative disorder. J Women Aging 2007;19:119–37. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]169. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition (DSM-5). Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing, 2013.170. Carlson, E. B. & Putnam, F. W. DES: Dissociative Experiences Scale II Accessed 4/20/2016.171. International Society for the Study of Trauma and Dissociative Disorders. Trauma and Dissociative Disorders FAQs Accessed 4/20/2016.172. Mental Health America. Dissociation and Dissociative Disorders Accessed 4/20/2016.173. Steinberg, M. Interviewers’ guide to the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Dissociative Disorders (SCID-D). Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing, 1994.I am here only to be truly helpful. I wish for you to create the life you deserve.
- Home >
- Catalog >
- Miscellaneous >
- Resume Template >
- Acting Resume Template >
- Sample Portfolio >
- design portfolio sample >
- Scoring Assistant Sample Report